Notes
Article history
The research reported in this issue of the journal was funded by the PHR programme as project number 09/3001/19. The contractual start date was in November 2009. The final report began editorial review in May 2013 and was accepted for publication December 2013. The authors have been wholly responsible for all data collection, analysis and interpretation, and for writing up their work. The PHR editors and production house have tried to ensure the accuracy of the authors’ report and would like to thank the reviewers for their constructive comments on the final report document. However, they do not accept liability for damages or losses arising from material published in this report.
Declared competing interests of authors
Janet E Cade is a personal member of the Royal Horticultural Society. Janet E Cade and Charlotte EL Evans received funding from Kids Company to undertake some subsequent analysis of this data set (results not presented in this report).
Permissions
Copyright statement
© Queen’s Printer and Controller of HMSO 2014. This work was produced by Christian et al. under the terms of a commissioning contract issued by the Secretary of State for Health. This issue may be freely reproduced for the purposes of private research and study and extracts (or indeed, the full report) may be included in professional journals provided that suitable acknowledgement is made and the reproduction is not associated with any form of advertising. Applications for commercial reproduction should be addressed to: NIHR Journals Library, National Institute for Health Research, Evaluation, Trials and Studies Coordinating Centre, Alpha House, University of Southampton Science Park, Southampton SO16 7NS, UK.
Chapter 1 Introduction
Fruit and vegetables are fundamental components of a healthy diet, providing vital micronutrients. The World Health Organization (WHO) recommends that we should eat at least 400 g of a variety of fruit and non-starchy vegetables every day. 1 A standard portion of fruit or vegetables is assumed to be 80 g. 1 Consuming low energy density foods such as vegetables could help prevent obesity. 2
Epidemiological evidence indicates that, in adults, a diet rich in fruit and vegetables can decrease the risk of developing cardiovascular disease, stroke, hypertension, type 2 diabetes mellitus, obesity and several forms of cancer. 3,4 A diet low in fruit and vegetables is one of the top 10 risk factors for global mortality. 5 Research has also revealed that dietary habits are developed in childhood and persist throughout life; therefore, it is vital that children at a young age consume adequate levels of fruit and vegetables. 6,7 Several studies indicate that children’s fruit and vegetable intake is positively associated with their parents’ intake. 8
The impact of poor nutrition in children is causing major public health concerns across the globe. 4 Of particular public concern is the rise of obesity in children. 9 Diet plays a fundamental role in weight management; having a healthy diet rich in fruit and vegetables, which have a low energy density, could help tackle the obesity epidemic. 10 A diet rich in fruit and vegetables is key for children to develop mentally and physically. 4 The importance of childhood eating patterns has been illustrated through longitudinal research concluding that eating fruit and vegetables in childhood has positive health benefits in terms of cardiovascular disease, asthma and other respiratory diseases. 11–14 Public health interventions need to change children’s lifestyles to reduce the intake of non-essential foods which are high in fat, sugar and salt, and to encourage increased intake of a variety of fruit and vegetables. Strategies to reduce obesity in children are urgently required. 15
Current consumption levels
Currently, children’s consumption of fruit and vegetables is low in the USA, Australia and most European countries. 12,16,17 The average intake of fruit and vegetables for children in the UK is around 2.8 servings per day – approximately 224 g. 18 In British children, the main sources of energy intake are chips, biscuits and crisps;19 the need for public health intervention to improve children’s overall diet habits is evident. 20 Children from low-income families consume even less fruit and vegetables than the average. Boys consume only 64 g, or 0.8 of a portion, and girls consume 1.1 portions (approximately 88 g) of fruit and vegetables daily. 19
Interventions to improve children’s fruit and vegetable intake from across the globe: what is already known?
Nutrition education programmes have been developed for school, home and community settings in an attempt to improve children’s diets. 21–28 Evidence suggests that the most effective interventions are multicomponent, with both school- and home-based components. 29–31 Successful intervention studies have included a variety of components: integrating teaching about fruit and vegetables into the curriculum;23,29,30,32–34 training teachers in theories of behaviour change and nutritional education;35,36 increasing fruit and vegetable availability at school and in school meals;34,36,37 training of catering staff (verbal encouragement);29,30,37 hands-on exposure (tasting and preparation sessions);23,33,35 parental involvement through newsletters and homework activities;23,29,30,33,34 a whole-school approach (developing a nutrition policy, evening activities)30,36,37 and community involvement (involving the local fruit and vegetable industry). 23,29,34 These intervention programmes report a moderate increase in children’s fruit and vegetable consumption of approximately one-third of a portion of fruit and/or vegetables per day. 22,38,39
The psychological theory behind school gardens is based on the social cognition theory (SCT), which works on the assumption that to successfully change a person’s behaviour requires changing their knowledge, values and beliefs. 40 It is believed that rather than being a quantitative effect, active engagement in gardening activities can reinforce healthy messages about eating, and increase children’s willingness to try different fruit and vegetables. Planting, growing and eating vegetables can improve children’s consumption patterns. However, there is now a gap between the implementation of school gardens and the academic evaluation of their effectiveness. Previous studies of school gardening are limited and none have been randomised controlled trials (RCTs). Studies have had issues with their design and the use of convenience sampling with relatively small sample sizes. 41–43 A number of the trials only had one school or club implementing the intervention. 41,44,45 Statistical analysis was also limited, with only three studies46–48 using a statistical methodology that adjusted for baseline differences. None of the studies took into consideration the hierarchical structures of school data through multilevel analysis. These factors could compromise reliability of statistical outcomes, limiting the generalisability of the results.
Potential barriers to changing children’s fruit and vegetable consumption
Changing children’s fruit and vegetable consumption is a challenging task. Academic literature shows that the main barriers to increasing children’s fruit and vegetable intake are availability, accessibility, convenience, taste preferences, peer pressure, parental/school support and knowledge. 49 The successful implementation of an intervention is often determined by the time allocated to the programme and the perception of its importance by teachers and parents. For teachers, the main barrier to implementing school-based interventions is preparation time. For parents, the cost of fruit and vegetables is often cited as being too high, with many opting to buy items of food that are less nutrient rich but are guaranteed to be consumed, such as biscuits, sweets and crisps. 50
Research has attempted to design complex interventions to improve the understanding and education of children regarding the importance of healthy eating. The complexity of these interventions is matched by the complexity of our relationship with food. Children’s desires, and their understanding and knowledge of nutrition, come not only from the school and family environments, but also from different types of media, supermarkets, packaging and television advertising, all of which influence their nutritional preferences. Literature suggests that in highly populated areas, such as inner cities, a gap has been created between children’s understanding of the processes of agriculture and the end result – the supermarket. 51,52 To increase children’s intake of fruit and vegetables, it is necessary to increase children’s general knowledge of fruit and vegetables. There is increasing evidence to suggest that gardening might be a vehicle for facilitating fruit and vegetable intake. 52–55
Barriers to implementing a school garden
School gardens require long-term commitment from the schools, and often need community assistance from parents if they are to be sustained. 54 Another issue found is that some schools under study took too long to establish their school gardens, affecting the period of time available during the studies for plants to germinate and grow edible fruit or vegetables. 54 Environmental factors will also play an important role in the amount of food harvested. Schools are closed over the summer, which is the peak harvesting season; without organising staff to water the garden and carry out general garden maintenance, the hard work during term time can be lost. The length of time spent in the interventions will also affect the chances of long-term change in children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Their consumption patterns are unlikely to be affected if their involvement in the actual intervention is limited.
The Royal Horticultural Society ‘Campaign for School Gardening’
This report describes two RCTs designed to evaluate an existing gardening programme run by the Royal Horticultural Society (RHS) in England. The RHS is the largest gardening charity in the UK and has existed for over 200 years. 56 The ‘Campaign for School Gardening’ programme was launched in 2007, and since then has recruited over 11,500 primary schools in England. The main aims of the programme are to encourage schools to be involved in growing fruit and vegetables, to enrich the curriculum activities of the schools and to educate children in the values of gardening, such as ‘healthy living’ and ’sustainability of the natural world’. 56 The RHS intervention is delivered using two different approaches: a trained RHS advisor or class teachers. The RHS advisor provides intensive, hands-on support to a small number of schools. The advisor also trains class teachers to develop the school garden in twilight after-school training sessions.
Figure 1, based on the work conducted by Krølner et al. ,57 illustrates the theoretical foundation for this study. It explores some of the factors that could assist or prevent the success of the intervention in affecting the primary outcome, highlighting important environmental, social and personal determinants that affect children’s nutritional behaviour. There are several determinants that are essential to changing a person’s health behaviour. 58 Without changing a child’s environment and access to fruit and vegetables, it would not be possible to change his or her overall intake. Watching parents, peers and teachers eating fruit and vegetables is pivotal in influencing children’s dietary habits and preferences. 59 In addition, nutrition education, presented in the form of a gardening intervention, should aim to increase children’s knowledge, creating the mechanisms necessary to increase overall intake. 60
Nevertheless, to be able to determine the effect of the intervention it is necessary to explore its implementation. The method by which the intervention is implemented, in this case delivered to the schools by the RHS advisor or conducted by the teacher, can have an influence on the primary outcome. Understanding the degree of implementation of the intervention in each school is fundamental in explaining the effect of the intervention. 61 Finally, the information in Figure 1 also illustrates the possible confounders (gender, age, ethnicity and socioeconomic status) that are associated with an effect on children’s fruit and vegetable intake.
Summary
School gardening programmes may provide an interactive environment with the potential to change children’s self-efficacy and willingness to try different fruit and vegetables. These changes in attitudes towards fruit and vegetables could lead to an increase in their actual consumption. Limitations of the existing research are the lack of RCTs and evaluation tools, and inadequate follow-up time. With the variability in quality of study design and validated tools to measure children’s nutritional intake, further research is needed to determine the potential impact gardening interventions have on children’s diets.
Chapter 2 Development and piloting of questionnaires
This chapter outlines the development of, modifications to and piloting of the data collection tools used in this study: the dietary assessment tool and DVD, child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire and the gardening process measures. It describes in detail the tools used to assess the primary and secondary outcome measures of the two trials. It also describes a pilot study conducted to confirm the suitability of language used in the questionnaires and to confirm the final data collection methodology for the trials.
The development of the data collection tools took place over two months from December 2009 until the end of January 2010. Ethical approval for the two trials was granted by the Leeds Institute of Health Sciences and the Leeds Institute of Genetics, Health and Therapeutics (LIHS/LIGHT) Joint Ethics Committee on 10 December 2009 (ref. number HSLT/09/012). The pilot study took place in two primary schools in Leeds in November 2009.
Primary outcome questionnaire
The Child And Diet Evaluation Tool
The primary aim of the two linked trials is to determine whether or not children who participate in the RHS advisor-led gardening intervention increase their fruit and vegetable consumption more than those who receive the teacher-led gardening intervention; or whether the teacher-led intervention increases their fruit and vegetable consumption more than no intervention at all. The effectiveness of either intervention (RHS advisor-led or teacher-led) will be determined by an increase in mean intake of one of the following: mean intake of fruit, mean intake of vegetables, or mean intake of fruit and vegetables at follow-up, after adjusting for baseline intake. Dietary intake, with a focus on fruit and vegetable intake, was measured using a modified version of the Child And Diet Evaluation Tool (CADET) questionnaire. 62 The main aim of the CADET diary is to collect accurate information on children’s fruit and vegetable intake, whilst also collecting information on all foods that the children consumed in a 24-hour period.
Part one of the CADET diary comprises a list of 115 separate food and drink types, divided into 15 categories. The categories of foods are cereal (five items); sandwich/bread/cake/biscuit (10 items); spreads/sauces/soup (seven items); cheese/egg (six items); chicken/turkey (three items); other meat (nine items); fish (five items); vegetarian (three items); pizza/pasta/rice (eight items); desserts/puddings (three items); sweets/crisps (four items); vegetables and beans (18 items); potato (two items); fruit (13 items); and drinks (nine items). Part two consists of food-related questions to identify daily consumption of milk, bread, sugar, spreads and fruit juice. It also includes general demographic questions about the family household, questions about the children’s and parents’ attitudes towards fruit and vegetables, and the availability of fruit and vegetables at home.
Data collection methodology
To complete the diary, participants tick each item consumed under the appropriate mealtime heading within the 24-hour period. In previous research with children aged 3–7 years, trained fieldworkers filled in the CADET diary during the school day, and parents were asked to complete the diary for evening and morning food consumption. 63,64 CADET has been validated for use in children aged 4–7 years in comparison with a semi-weighed food diary collected on a school day, but it has not been used in children aged 8–11 years, the age group of children in this study. After evaluation of previous studies,63,65 the following modifications to the data collection methodology were made:
-
The CADET diary was split into two: a school diary to record all food consumed at school, and a home diary to record all food consumed at home. These two versions of CADET were renamed as the school food diary and home food diary.
-
Additional demographic questions were added to explore the home food environment.
-
On the day after the food-recording day, the fieldworker went back to the school to collect the home food diary, and checked that it had been completed accurately. If a child forgot to return the home food diary a retrospective recall was taken by the fieldworker to record all evening meals and breakfast.
Justification for these changes came from the response rate of a previous study, ‘Project Tomato’. 66 At baseline of Project Tomato, 3159 children took part in the study. Of these children, 280 never returned the CADET diary after it was sent home to be completed by their parents. This meant that the data collected during the whole day were lost, and no data were collected on these children. An additional 170 CADET diaries sent home to be completed by parents were returned without any of the sections completed. This reduced the sample size to 2709 – a loss of 450 children or 14%. Furthermore, when analysing the data collected from this study, children with a total energy intake of < 500 kcal or > 3500 kcal on the day of CADET administration were excluded. This led to a further 179 children being excluded. It was anticipated that some of these errors in data collection would be rectified after splitting the CADET diary into two diaries and having the fieldworker revisit the school to check that the home food diary had been completed.
Portion sizes for children aged 8–11 years
The dietary information from the CADET diary was transferred to a Microsoft Access spreadsheet (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, WA, USA) using our established in-house software, named Diet And Nutrition Tool for Evaluation (DANTE) (Nutritional Epidemiology Group, Leeds). This used standard predefined algorithms to convert food items into total daily nutrient values for each child, based on the composition of foods. 67 Although the CADET diary upon which the school and home food diaries were based has previously been validated in children aged 3–7 years, it has not been used to collect dietary information in 8- to 11-year-olds. As this study includes children aged 7–10 years, it was necessary to change the standard portion sizes in DANTE to reflect the children’s intake for each year of age (i.e. 8, 9, 10 and 11 years), and to account for differences in intake for boys and girls.
Methodology
Protocol for determining portion sizes for children aged 8, 9, 10 and 11 years
The portion sizes for ages 8, 9, 10 and 11 years were obtained from the National Diet and Nutrition Survey (NDNS) of young people aged 4–18 years. 68 The NDNS was conducted to explore food consumption and nutrient intake in the general population living in privately owned houses across Britain. The NDNS data are based on an interview and a 4-day food diary as well as blood and urine samples. The NDNS is the most detailed nutrition survey conducted across Britain. A recent update of the report (2008/09–2009/10) confirmed that the overall diet intake was similar to the previous findings. Owing to the validity of these data, it was decided that they would be used to update the CADET portion sizes for older children. 18
From the NDNS data, the mean portion size, number of participants, standard deviation (SD), and maximum and minimum values were extracted. Nearly all the food items used in the CADET were available from the NDNS data and were then further broken down into each age category by gender. Whereas commonly consumed items such as apples and bananas were consumed by, on average, a higher number of participants in each age group (32 and 24 children on average per age group, respectively), several items were consumed by, on average, fewer than five participants per age group. For these foods, the portions had notably higher variation compared with those foods with a higher number of observations. The likelihood that these portion sizes reflected those of the general population that consumed them was questionable. Furthermore, some food items, once broken down into age/gender categories, were found to have missing data. To improve the validity of those foods with low or missing numbers of participants, the rules in the following sections were applied.
Missing data
If any foods included in the CADET dairy were not available as specific codes/items on the NDNS database, then a similar food item was substituted.
Food items with, on average, fewer than five participants per age/gender category
If the item was consumed by fewer than five participants, on average, per age/gender category, an appropriate nutritionally similar food with an average of 10 or more participants per age/gender category would be obtained. The average of the two means would then be calculated in an attempt to reflect actual intake for each category.
An example of this is kiwi, which had, on average, only one person per age/gender category. It also had no value for girls aged 8 years. For kiwi, an average of kiwi and peach, nectarine, plum, apricot and mango was used to ensure a better representation of the average portion sizes for the different age groups consuming them, based on gender. The aim was to smooth out the data where there were extreme values based on one person, and to gain a more valid estimation of intake. For each food that was changed, a line graph was produced containing both the pre-existing food, for example ‘kiwi’, and the modified food, for example ‘average of kiwi and peach, nectarine, plum, apricot and mango’, to visually confirm that the portion sizes looked appropriate. The reason for doing so was to confirm the direction of change in consumption, as at different ages and for different foods, children can not only increase, but also decrease their consumption. Tables 1 and 2 along with Figures 2 and 3 show the portion sizes for children aged 3–11 years, to demonstrate the overall change in portion sizes by age. The calculations of portion sizes as consumed were made only for children aged 8–11 years.
Food item | Portion size (g) | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age 3 years | Age 4 years | Age 5 years | Age 6 years | Age 7 years | Age 8 years | Age 9 years | Age 10 years | Age 11 years | |
Kiwi fruit (n = 8) | 46 | 74 | 43 | 70 | 63 | 93 | 68 | 75 | 0 |
Peach, nectarine, plum, apricot and mango (n = 21) | 55 | 86 | 63 | 109 | 101 | 79 | 68 | 78 | 48 |
Average (n = 29) | 46 | 74 | 43 | 70 | 63 | 86 | 68 | 76 | 48 |
Food item | Portion size (g) | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age 3 years | Age 4 years | Age 5 years | Age 6 years | Age 7 years | Age 8 years | Age 9 years | Age 10 years | Age 11 years | |
Kiwi fruit (n = 4) | 60 | 94 | 26 | 68 | 120 | 76 | 72 | 0 | 72 |
Peach, nectarine, plum, apricot and mango (n = 23) | 92 | 80 | 94 | 69 | 83 | 79 | 60 | 48 | 60 |
Average (n = 27) | 76 | 87 | 60 | 69 | 102 | 74 | 66 | 48 | 69 |
Modifications
Of the 115 foods in the school and home food diaries, 21 had no dietary examples from the NDNS data and 16 had an average sample size of fewer than five participants. Table 3 lists the food items from CADET that did not have a NDNS portion size, and the food groups used as a substitute to create an appropriate portion size for consumers. Table 4 lists the foods with an average of five or fewer participants per age group in the NDNS data, and the food groups that were used as a substitute.
Food group | Substitute food portion | Portion size (g) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Boys aged 8 years | Girls aged 8 years | Boys aged 9 years | Girls aged 9 years | Boys aged 10 years | Girls aged 10 years | Boys aged 11 years | Girls aged 11 years | ||
Chapatti/pitta bread/wrap/roti | Bread sticks and garlic bread/naan/paratha | 42 | 23 | 32 | 44 | 31 | 52 | 26 | 45 |
Cottage cheese | Cheese spread, triangles | 26 | 22 | 24 | 22 | 26 | 23 | 36 | 25 |
Chicken in a creamy sauce | Other meats: stew, casserole, mince, curry or keema | 139 | 159 | 144 | 118 | 115 | 115 | 164 | 151 |
Vegetable pie/pasty | Sausage roll, meat pie, pasty, fried dumplings | 118 | 126 | 126 | 127 | 138 | 122 | 148 | 143 |
Samosa/pakora/bhajee | Vegetable pie, pasty | 118 | 126 | 126 | 127 | 138 | 122 | 148 | 143 |
Quorn™/vegetarian mince/sausages | Sausage roll, meat pie, pasty, fried dumplings | 66 | 69 | 64 | 69 | 77 | 69 | 82 | 72 |
Paneer (cheese curry) | Other meats: stew, casserole, mince, curry or keema | 139 | 159 | 144 | 118 | 115 | 115 | 167 | 151 |
Fried rice | Boiled rice | 132 | 100 | 143 | 120 | 151 | 134 | 172 | 122 |
Pasta with meat/fish and sauce | Average of pasta with a cheese sauce and pasta with tomato sauce | 164 | 155 | 170 | 231 | 238 | 160 | 144 | 185 |
Stir-fried vegetables | Average of carrots, cauliflower and peas | 46 | 47 | 53 | 56 | 67 | 63 | 61 | 53 |
Courgettes | Average of carrots, cauliflower and peas | 46 | 47 | 53 | 56 | 67 | 63 | 61 | 53 |
Spinach | Lettuces | 19 | 16 | 21 | 35 | 25 | 22 | 22 | 28 |
Parsnips | Carrots | 44 | 38 | 44 | 52 | 55 | 54 | 49 | 47 |
Radishes | Average of peppers and salad | 11 | 11 | 12 | 10 | 24 | 22 | 26 | 36 |
Leeks | Onions | 15 | 15 | 15 | 28 | 26 | 23 | 35 | 20 |
Other vegetables | Average of carrots, cauliflower and peas/sweetcorn | 46 | 47 | 53 | 56 | 67 | 63 | 61 | 53 |
Lentils/dahl | Peas and sweetcorn | 42 | 36 | 59 | 55 | 69 | 46 | 59 | 52 |
Other beans | Brussels sprouts | 42 | 36 | 59 | 55 | 69 | 46 | 59 | 52 |
Pineapples | Grapes | 84 | 58 | 61 | 105 | 40 | 90 | 85 | 90 |
Other fresh fruit | Peaches, nectarines, plums, apricots, mangoes and average of strawberries/grapes | 100 | 92 | 111 | 72 | 88 | 89 | 71 | 97 |
Mousse/milk/rice puddings | Custard | 97 | 91 | 109 | 105 | 112 | 80 | 146 | 104 |
Food group | Substitute food | Portion size (g) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Boys aged 8 years | Girls aged 8 years | Boys aged 9 years | Girls aged 9 years | Boys aged 10 years | Girls aged 10 years | Boys aged 11 years | Girls aged 11 years | ||
Croissants/waffles/Pop-Tarts® | Crumpets/pikelets/scotch pancakes | 52 | 54 | 52 | 53 | 67 | 57 | 65 | 52 |
Nuts | Dried fruit | 35 | 54 | 23 | 36 | 31 | 38 | 49 | 34 |
Quiche | Sausage roll, meat pie, pasty, fried dumplings | 77 | 80 | 77 | 79 | 76 | 81 | 79 | 84 |
Corned beef, luncheon meats/salami | Ham | 52 | 31 | 32 | 39 | 64 | 52 | 49 | 40 |
White fish (not fried) | Fish in breadcrumbs and fishcakes | 80 | 78 | 112 | 81 | 83 | 79 | 83 | 62 |
Shellfish, e.g. prawns/mussels | Tuna and other oily fish | 44 | 33 | 56 | 50 | 54 | 54 | 45 | 47 |
Offal | Ham | 24 | 18 | 28 | 27 | 63 | 47 | 38 | 39 |
Celery | Salad vegetables | 27 | 8 | 17 | 33 | 24 | 22 | 26 | 36 |
Peppers (red, green, yellow) | Salad vegetables | 15 | 8 | 15 | 38 | 23 | 19 | 16 | 25 |
Strawberries/raspberries | Grapes | 107 | 104 | 128 | 105 | 93 | 99 | 82 | 90 |
Pears | Apples | 130 | 123 | 127 | 95 | 108 | 114 | 123 | 115 |
Melons/watermelons | Bananas | 199 | 167 | 138 | 220 | 171 | 133 | 102 | 140 |
Kiwi fruits | Peaches, nectarines, plums, apricots, mangoes | 76 | 87 | 60 | 69 | 102 | 77 | 66 | 48 |
Sugar-coated cereals | High-fibre cereals, e.g. bran flakes, Weetabix®, Shreddies® | 43 | 40 | 18 | 36 | 17 | 38 | 46 | 46 |
Table 5 shows the final portion sizes for all vegetables, as used in DANTE for the CADET diaries, for boys and girls across the different age groups. Overall, there is a general trend towards a small increase in vegetable consumption for both boys and girls. However, there is more variability between the different ages for both boys and girls in fruit intake (Table 6). Melon and watermelon portion sizes vary greatly between year groups; this could be a consequence of the infrequent consumption of both of these fruits. It was decided not to overmanipulate the data and to leave these portion sizes as they are, as the NDNS data are based on weighed intakes from a nationally representative sample.
Food | Portion size (g) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Boys aged 8 years | Girls aged 8 years | Boys aged 9 years | Girls aged 9 years | Boys aged 10 years | Girls aged 10 years | Boys aged 11 years | Girls aged 11 years | |
Baked beans | 95 | 97 | 112 | 97 | 113 | 92 | 104 | 104 |
Broccoli, Brussels sprouts, cabbages | 50 | 59 | 53 | 52 | 68 | 56 | 60 | 67 |
Carrots | 42 | 32 | 40 | 58 | 54 | 56 | 41 | 42 |
Cauliflowers | 52 | 66 | 54 | 61 | 78 | 88 | 75 | 61 |
Celery | 27 | 8 | 17 | 33 | 24 | 22 | 26 | 36 |
Coleslaw | 47 | 44 | 47 | 35 | 42 | 38 | 64 | 42 |
Courgettes | 46 | 47 | 53 | 56 | 67 | 63 | 61 | 53 |
Cucumbers | 32 | 27 | 25 | 34 | 23 | 31 | 25 | 28 |
Leeks | 15 | 15 | 15 | 28 | 26 | 23 | 35 | 20 |
Lentils, dahl | 42 | 36 | 59 | 55 | 69 | 46 | 59 | 52 |
Mixed vegetables | 42 | 36 | 59 | 55 | 69 | 46 | 46 | 52 |
Other beans, pulses | 42 | 36 | 59 | 55 | 69 | 46 | 59 | 52 |
Other salad vegetables | 19 | 16 | 21 | 35 | 25 | 22 | 22 | 28 |
Other vegetables | 46 | 47 | 53 | 56 | 67 | 63 | 61 | 53 |
Parsnips | 44 | 38 | 44 | 52 | 55 | 54 | 49 | 47 |
Peas, sweetcorn | 42 | 36 | 59 | 55 | 69 | 46 | 59 | 52 |
Peppers (red, green, yellow, etc.) | 15 | 8 | 15 | 38 | 23 | 19 | 16 | 25 |
Radishes | 11 | 11 | 12 | 10 | 24 | 22 | 26 | 36 |
Spinach | 19 | 16 | 21 | 35 | 25 | 22 | 22 | 28 |
Stir-fried vegetables | 46 | 47 | 53 | 56 | 67 | 63 | 61 | 53 |
Tomatoes | 69 | 75 | 24 | 64 | 33 | 69 | 47 | 41 |
Food | Portion size (g) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Boys aged 8 years | Girls aged 8 years | Boys aged 9 years | Girls aged 9 years | Boys aged 10 years | Girls aged 10 years | Boys aged 11 years | Girls aged 11 years | |
Apples | 123 | 117 | 117 | 122 | 120 | 114 | 123 | 115 |
Bananas | 104 | 93 | 110 | 119 | 114 | 119 | 102 | 116 |
Dried fruit | 35 | 54 | 23 | 36 | 31 | 38 | 49 | 67 |
Fruit salad (tinned or fresh) | 107 | 104 | 128 | 105 | 93 | 99 | 82 | 90 |
Grapes | 84 | 58 | 61 | 105 | 40 | 90 | 85 | 90 |
Kiwi fruits | 76 | 87 | 60 | 69 | 102 | 77 | 66 | 48 |
Melons | 199 | 167 | 138 | 220 | 171 | 133 | 102 | 140 |
Oranges, satsumas, etc. | 132 | 160 | 117 | 137 | 105 | 140 | 84 | 57 |
Other fruit | 100 | 92 | 111 | 72 | 88 | 89 | 71 | 97 |
Peaches, nectarines, plums, apricots, mangoes | 92 | 80 | 94 | 69 | 83 | 79 | 60 | 48 |
Pears | 130 | 123 | 127 | 95 | 108 | 114 | 123 | 115 |
Pineapples | 84 | 58 | 61 | 105 | 40 | 90 | 85 | 90 |
Strawberries, raspberries, etc. | 107 | 104 | 128 | 105 | 93 | 99 | 82 | 90 |
Additional demographic questions
There were several questions added to part two of the CADET diary. The first set of questions explored the availability of fruit and vegetables at home, and children’s and parents’ fruit and vegetable consumption habits. An example question is ‘do you buy a specific fruit/vegetable because your child asked for it?’ The parents were presented with the response options of ‘yes, always’, ‘yes, most days/often’, ’sometimes’, ‘rarely’ and ‘never’. These questions were based on the existing literature. 69 This research explored the availability and accessibility of fruit and vegetables in the home environment. Test–retest reliability was conducted in five different European countries: Norway, Spain, Denmark, Portugal and Belgium. The intraclass correlation coefficient was 0.6, suggesting that it is a reliable tool to use in primary school-aged children. 70
A second set of six questions was added regarding the family’s fruit and vegetable intake. These questions were developed from a previous study,71 and addressed parents’ reasons for buying fruit and vegetables for their families and the importance of buying fruit and vegetables. One such question addressed the ‘price of fruit and vegetables’; parents were provided with the options of ‘very important’, ‘important’, ‘neither unimportant or important’, ‘unimportant’ and ‘very unimportant’.
The third and final question that was added to the home food diary was a request for an inventory of fruit and vegetables that were in the house on the evening the diary was completed. This question listed common fruit and vegetables consumed and asked the parents to tick any that were currently in their food cupboards or fridge. There was also a section designated ‘other’ for any items that were not listed.
These questions in part two were included to provide some understanding of the home food environment, providing us with insight into the availability of fruit and vegetables, and parents’ attitudes towards fruit and vegetables. This insight is crucial, as the home food environment is a key influence on children’s food intake. 72
Development of the home food diary instruction DVD
Previous studies that have implemented the CADET diary to measure dietary intake have identified that parents and children with low literacy or English as a second language struggle to complete the diary. 64 Although the CADET diary had two pages of simple instructions on how to complete it, it was evident that some participants still did not understand what was required of them. Sometimes children or parents would complete the diary, ticking every item in the diary that the child liked, rather than only those foods that the child had eaten that day. Some did not complete the CADET at all, and simply did not return it to the school, despite several reminders. To improve accuracy and completion of the home food diary, the concept of creating an instructional DVD for parents and children to watch was developed (Figure 4).
The DVD script was written with the aim of encouraging children and parents to watch the DVD together. It used a cartoon character to explain each step of the diary, while showing the actual diary on-screen for parents and children to follow. The script was written by MSC with input from PhD supervisors, with the aim of introducing the study to the audience and explaining how to complete the home food diary, step by step. The main aims of the script were to introduce the study, remind the children to make sure their parents were watching, demonstrate how to complete the diary for each meal, explain what ‘part two’ questions consisted of and remind participants to return the diary to school the next day. It also provided a contact number for parents to ring if they had any queries.
It was decided that a cartoon character would be the narrator and would resemble the characters used by the Nintendo Wii™ console. The cartoon character was a tomato called ‘Tom the Tomato’, which had a head like a tomato and a red body, alive in a plant pot. The concept behind using a cartoon character was to make the DVD child-friendly so that hopefully children and parents would find it interesting. To construct the DVD, Leeds Media Service were contracted. It was decided that the voice of Tom the Tomato would be a child’s voice, and Emily Cade, who was 16 years of age, was recruited as she had a clear speaking voice with very little regional accent. The total length of the instruction DVD is 5 minutes. The end product is a useful tool for anyone completing the home food diary questionnaire as it both ‘verbally tells you’ and ‘visually shows you’ how to be complete the form (see https://youtube.googleapis.com/v/AIbzqaJiHq0%26hl=en_US%26fs=1%26rel=0%26hd=1).
Secondary outcome questionnaires
Development of the questionnaire on knowledge and attitudes towards fruit and vegetables
One of the secondary outcome measures was ‘Can participating in a school gardening intervention improve children’s ability to identify specific fruit and vegetables and their attitudes towards fruit and vegetables?’
Since the RHS gardening intervention is an educational resource that teaches children about fruit and vegetables through gardening, it has the potential to have an impact on children’s general knowledge of fruit and vegetables. Therefore, one of the other main aims of this study was to explore change in children’s knowledge and attitudes towards fruit and vegetables, to see if there was a difference from baseline to follow-up. A short questionnaire was developed to identify children’s knowledge and attitudes towards fruit and vegetable consumption, and to assess gardening activity levels (see Appendix 1, Child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire). The knowledge questions assessed children’s ability to recognise different fruit and vegetables. Children were presented with a list of 13 fruits, 17 vegetables and one herb, with a colour picture for each, and were asked to draw a line connecting each name with the right picture. The attitude questions were based on previously validated research. 73 Children were asked if they agreed or disagreed with ideas about fruit and vegetables, and were presented with a list of 10 statements, five of these about fruit and five about vegetables. An example is ‘I enjoy eating fruit’. The children had to circle one of four options: ‘agree a lot’, ‘agree a little’, ‘disagree a lot’ or ‘disagree a little’. Images of smiley, neutral or sad faces were presented above each statement to help the children work out their response.
The gardening questions assessed the children’s gardening experience, in terms of what they have grown and what they have tasted. The children were asked to confirm if they had done any gardening (‘yes’ or ‘no’) and then write in the space provided if they had grown any fruit or vegetables. They were then asked to confirm if they had tasted any of the fruit or vegetables they had grown (‘yes’ or ‘no’) and to write down what they had tasted.
To assist with the varying levels of reading ability, this questionnaire was read out to the children as a class, to help them with any difficult words. Furthermore, the teachers and teaching assistants were encouraged to help those children who might struggle with this task, and children were encouraged to put their hands up if they had any questions.
Process measures questionnaires
There were two process measures components for this study; the first was a gardening telephone interview, to identify current level of gardening activities within the school (see Appendix 1); the second was a gardening activity process measures questionnaire to identify the gardening activities that had taken place in each academic year in each school.
School gardening questionnaire
The school gardening questionnaire was a telephone interview. It was designed to identify the school’s baseline gardening level. This questionnaire was based on the RHS benchmarking scheme, which ranks schools in the following categories: (1) planning, (2) getting started, (3) growing and diversifying, (4) sharing best practice and (5) celebrating with the wider community. The schools were asked a series of questions to identify different aspects of gardening currently occurring in their school garden. The questions were focused on the following aspects of gardening in schools: school culture and ethos, the school garden, teaching and learning, and community. Within each of these areas there were several questions that reflected different levels of development within school gardening relating to the five stages of developing a school garden. These questions were adapted as simple ‘yes or no’ questions to be used in a telephone interview. The interviewee was the school staff member who was most involved in the school garden within each school. The questions were structured according to the five categories.
Gardening activity process measures questionnaire
The aim of the gardening activity questionnaire was to identify the level of adherence to the intervention by the schools involved, and to identify any gardening activities that are being undertaken by the control schools. The main aim of the process evaluations was to capture which fruit and vegetables each school grows and harvests. They also aimed to identify which year groups had been involved in the garden each year, whether or not they had started a growing or environmental club, and to find out if the schools had any success or failure stories around the school garden. This information was captured via e-mail in September 2010 for trial years 1 and 2, and again in September 2011 for both trials.
For the schools involved in the RHS intervention, more in-depth information about their intervention activities was captured by the regional advisor and was used to outline changes in school gardening. From this, the level of involvement in the intervention by each school and their adherence to the intervention was identified, as well as success and failure stories reported by the regional advisor himself.
For trial 2 intervention schools, another process measure captured was the level of involvement in the twilight sessions, whereby the regional advisor kept a record of the teacher’s attendance. With this type of intervention, schools were expected to tailor the intervention to their individual needs. By monitoring what activities are undertaken in the school garden, aspects of the intervention that could be associated with dietary change were identified.
Questionnaire development summary
The main aim of the dietary assessment tool was to collect information on children’s fruit and vegetable intake, while also collecting information on all the food the children consumed in one 24-hour period. Whereas one 24-hour food diary has been used previously, for this study CADET was changed and modified into a school food diary and a home food diary, to improve the response rate for the home food diary. Furthermore, the portion sizes used to analyse the children’s food intake were changed to reflect the age- and gender-related portion sizes of the sample. A DVD was also designed to help parents and children understand how to complete the home food diary. The final modification was a change in the administration of the diaries, with the fieldworkers returning to each school the day after collection, to collate and check the diaries and to identify any that had not been completed properly. An additional step was to collect a dietary recall of food and drink consumed at home from children who had not returned their diaries that day.
To ensure that these portion sizes reflected actual dietary intake, it was necessary to test this instrument – the home and school food diaries – against an appropriate reference measurement, such as a 1-day weighed record in children of the relevant ages in Years 3, 4, 5 and 6. 19,74
Additional questionnaires were designed to measure the secondary outcome measures for this study:
-
a child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire
-
a gardening telephone interview questionnaire
-
a gardening in schools process measures questionnaire.
These questionnaires were designed to capture important information to evaluate the effectiveness of the RHS gardening intervention, through evaluating children’s learning and knowledge with a focus on fruit and vegetables, capturing change in schools’ gardening involvement based on RHS gardening levels and assessing implementation of the intervention or other gardening activities in schools. Examples of all questionnaires can be found in Appendix 1.
Piloting baseline materials
Owing to the changes made to the original CADET diary, the collection method and the development of the new questionnaires (including the child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire) as well as the instruction DVD, it was necessary to pilot these materials. Two primary schools in West Yorkshire were recruited to be involved in a pilot study of the collection procedure and the new materials, namely the school and home food diaries, the child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire, the school gardening questionnaire and the instructional DVD.
The aims and objectives of the pilot study were:
-
to determine whether the DVD should be shown in the classroom at school, or sent home with the children for them and their parents to watch together
-
to confirm that the questionnaire was age-appropriate in terms of language used and layout, and to identify whether or not there were any questions that children struggled to answer
-
to test the new data collection protocol and explore the potential benefits of having the fieldworkers check the home food diary the following day.
Methodology
Study population
A total of 74 Year 3 and 4 children from two local primary schools in Leeds (mean age 8.4 years) participated in the pilot study. This involved three different class groups: one Year 3, one Year 4 and a mixed Year 3 and 4 class. To evaluate whether the DVD should be sent home or viewed in school, one class was allocated to receive the DVD to watch in class, another was allocated to be given the DVD to take home and the third class was allocated not to receive the DVD at all.
Masters students in nutrition were recruited and trained to administer the CADET diaries and the child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire. The students were asked to record everything the children ate at school by completing the school food diary, and then to go through the child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire as a class. At the end of the school day, one class of children was given the home food diary, one class was asked to watch the DVD before they were given the home food diary, and the final class was given the DVD and the home food diary and asked to watch the DVD with their parents.
Results
A total of 74 children were invited to participate in the pilot study, of which 62 parents agreed to let their children participate. The results from this study are presented in Table 7.
Allocation of DVD | Response rate, n (%) | Boys, n (%) | Year level | Returned the home food diary, n (%) | Home food diary recalls, n (%) | 5 A DAY ‘correct’ answer, n (%)a |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Received the DVD to take home (n = 33) | 30 (90) | 13 (43) | 3 and 4 | 25 (83) | 5 (17) | 19 (63) |
Watched the DVD at school (n = 22) | 15 (68) | 9 (60) | 3 | 11 (73) | 4 (27) | 8 (53) |
No DVD given (n = 19) | 17 (89) | 10 (59) | 4 | 8 (42) | 9 (53) | 9 (53) |
Home food diary and instruction DVD
One of the aims of the pilot study was to evaluate whether the DVD should be shown in the classroom at school, or sent home with the children for them and their parents to watch together; there were concerns about children forgetting to return the DVD to the school the next day, and losing the DVD. The results indicated that children who received the DVD to take home and watch with their parents had a higher home food diary return rate (83%) compared with those who watched the DVD in class (73%) or did not receive the DVD (52%). Of those parents who confirmed that they had watched the DVD, all completed the home food diary correctly. Therefore, it was decided that all children should receive the DVD to take home and watch with their parents to improve the quality of the data collected.
School food diary
The fieldworkers were also required to complete the school food diary for all the children in the pilot study. It was brought to our attention that Yorkshire pudding was not included in the school food diary, as one school had it as part of its school dinners; it was then added to both the school and home food diaries. There was also a comment from one of the parents about the home food diary; they stated that they would prefer their ethnicity to be classified as ‘British Asian’ rather than ‘Asian British’. This was rectified.
Data collection protocol
On the second day of data collection, the fieldworkers had two tasks: (1) to check that the home food diary was completed properly, and (2) to complete a diet recall for those children who did not return the home food diary. These results reveal that 25% of the total sample did not return the home food diary. Of the children who were allocated to watch the DVD with their parents, only 17% needed a diet recall to be taken, compared with 27% of those who watched the DVD at school and 42% of those who did not receive the DVD.
Knowledge and attitudes questionnaire results
To assist with the psychological questions and the variability in children’s reading ability, the knowledge and attitudes questionnaire was read aloud to the children and completed together as a class. Teachers were encouraged to assist any children who they thought might struggle with completing the questionnaire.
Administration of the questionnaires was successfully completed. There were six different sections in the child questionnaire. There was only one section which children struggled to complete; this was section 4, containing psychological questions about gardening and fruit and vegetable self-efficacy. Children were asked to respond ‘agree a lot’, ‘agree a little’, ‘disagree a little’ or ‘disagree a lot’ to each of these questions (presented in Table 8). In view of the feedback from fieldworkers, five of the questions were removed. Furthermore, a smiley face or sad face was added under the different options (‘agree a little’, etc.) to help children choose how to respond to each of these questions.
Question | Question removed? |
---|---|
I like trying new fruits | No |
I like trying new veg | No |
Eating fruit and veg every day keeps me healthy | No |
Most fruit tastes bad | Yes |
We have veg with dinner most nights | Yes |
There’s usually lots of fruit and veg snacks at home | Yes |
I’m good at preparing fruit and veg | No |
I like raw veg | Yes |
We grow fruit or veg at home | Yes |
My parents encourage me to eat fruit and veg | No |
I enjoy eating fruit | No |
I enjoy eating vegetables | No |
I try to eat lots of fruit | No |
I try to eat lots of vegetables | No |
I find it easy to eat lots of fruit | No |
I find it easy to eat lots of vegetables | No |
The results also revealed that, on average, when asked how many fruit and vegetables one should eat every day to stay healthy, 52% of the children were not aware that they should consume at least five portions of fruit and vegetables a day.
Discussion
Accurately measuring children’s energy and nutrient intake is challenging, especially in a large trial such as this, as there are always benefits and limitations of any nutritional assessment tool. Research suggests that children are aware of what they consume from around 8 years old. 75 For primary school-aged children, parents are often used to collect the dietary information as the children themselves are considered too young to collect accurate dietary data. However, dietary analysis is prone to many forms of measurement error. 76 CADET has been validated in an ethnically diverse population62 and has been used to evaluate large intervention studies. These include the national free school fruit scheme in primary school children,63 and a large national RCT of an intervention to maintain fruit and vegetable eating in Year 3 children once they are no longer eligible for free fruit. 77,78 The style of CADET, using a simple tick-box list, is considered an appropriate tool for people with low literacy who struggle to record or weigh what they eat. The main benefit of using a 24-hour tool is that it is easy to complete in a large sample at a relatively low cost. 79 This style of nutrition analysis will capture the mean intake of a population, and is the standard method used for intervention evaluation. The disadvantage of 24-hour data is that they cannot be used to analyse individual intake, as the instrument is not sensitive enough to identify individual differences in dietary patterns. 50,79 Nevertheless, CADET has been proven to be a valid tool for evaluating intervention studies in trials,62,64,77 and it is an effective way to capture fruit and vegetable intake in children.
There were three main aims of the pilot study. The first was to determine whether the children should take the DVD home to watch, or watch it at school. The results revealed that children and parents who watched the DVD together had a higher response rate than children who watched the DVD at home on their own, or who did not watch the DVD at all. The second aim was to test the child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire, to confirm that the questionnaire was age-appropriate in terms of language used and to identify whether or not there were any questions that children struggled to answer. This identified that children struggled with some of the psychological questions, such as ‘We have veg with dinner most nights’ and ‘There’s usually lots of fruit and veg snacks at home’; therefore, these questions were removed. The final aim of the pilot study was to test the new protocol methodology. On the second day of data collection, 18 (25%) of the children did not bring back a home food diary; if the fieldworkers had not conducted a recall, then 25% of the sample diet data would have been lost. The fieldworkers also provided positive comments regarding conducting the diet recall. This is supported by other research which states that children are aware of what they consume from around 8 years of age, the mean age of the trial children. 80
Overall, the aims of the pilot study were achieved, and the results were able to provide important feedback in the development of the necessary tools needed to evaluate the RHS gardening intervention.
Summary
This chapter has discussed the methodology used in designing the data collection tools for this study. It also discussed the pilot study conducted in Leeds and the changes made as a consequence of this process.
The pilot study revealed that it was beneficial for parents to watch the DVD at home with their children, when compared with children who watched the DVD at school or not at all. It also highlights some of the psychological questions that children in Years 3 and 4 struggled to understand, and some minor changes made to the food diaries. These changes and additions to the collection methodology aim to improve the overall response rate and quality of the data collected.
Chapter 3 Methodology
This chapter outlines the general methodological components that applied to both trials. It will discuss:
-
sampling and recruitment of schools (inclusion and exclusion criteria)
-
sample size calculation
-
randomisation methodology
-
training of fieldworkers to collect the baseline and follow-up data
-
the two interventions: RHS-led and teacher-led
-
data cleaning methodology.
It should also be noted that this chapter is based on a published protocol written by MSC. 81
Sampling and recruitment of schools
It is RHS policy to provide support to all schools that register an interest in the campaign. As a consequence of this, two linked trials were required. All schools in the London boroughs supported by the RHS (Tower Hamlets, Greenwich, Wandsworth and Sutton) would be given access to either the regional advisor or twilight teacher training sessions. These boroughs represent two relatively deprived areas and two less deprived regions in London. A second set of schools from adjacent boroughs was recruited by the research team into trial 2 and randomised to receive the twilight teacher training or no RHS gardening intervention.
Addresses of all schools were supplied by the local education authority of the nominated London boroughs for each trial; the schools were then sent a recruitment letter (see example provided in Appendix 2). Schools were asked to reply, providing information on their gardening activities. These responses were checked by both the University of Leeds team and the RHS Campaign for School Gardening manager before randomising the schools to one of the interventions or the comparison group.
Trial 1: Royal Horticultural Society-led intervention versus teacher-led intervention
The RHS introduced its Campaign for School Gardening to schools in the London region in the autumn of 2009. The RHS campaign provided intensive support to 10 schools in each region through support from an RHS School Gardening Regional Advisor (the RHS-led intervention). The remaining schools had access to support through twilight training sessions for staff and other activities (the teacher-led intervention). A sample size of 10 schools received the RHS-led intervention, as this was the maximum number of schools that could be supported by one regional advisor. Further details of the intervention components are discussed later in this chapter.
Twenty-six schools from four boroughs in London (Wandsworth, Tower Hamlets, Greenwich and Sutton) were recruited for trial 1. Of the 26 schools, 10 were randomly allocated to receive the RHS-led and 16 to receive the teacher-led intervention. The allocation sequence was generated using Stata Version 11 (StataCorp LP, College Station, TX, USA). All schools were allocated at the same time. It was not possible to randomise schools in trial 1 to receive no intervention at all owing to RHS policy.
Rationale for trial 2
In trial 1 it was not possible to randomise schools to receive no intervention at all (control/comparison group) as it is RHS policy to provide support to all schools who register an interest in the campaign. As a consequence of this, the second set of schools was recruited into a linked trial, trial 2, to provide a ‘no intervention’ arm, i.e. a comparison group.
Trial 2: teacher-led versus delayed intervention
Thirty-two schools from four boroughs in London (Lewisham, Lambeth, Merton and Newham) were recruited for trial 2. These boroughs are adjacent to the trial 1 boroughs. Of these schools, 16 were randomly allocated to receive the teacher-led intervention and 16 were used as comparison schools. The comparison schools received no active intervention during the trial. However, they were informed that once the study ended follow-up collection in February 2012, they would be able to attend the twilight sessions offered to the teacher-led schools.
It was not possible to blind the schools to their intervention group because of the nature of the intervention. The fieldworkers were blinded to the allocation of schools to the intervention (RHS-led or teacher-led) and comparison arms of the study.
Study population
Trial 1 inclusion criteria
All non-fee-paying primary schools within four London boroughs (Wandsworth, Tower Hamlets, Greenwich and Sutton) with classes in Key Stage 2 (Years 3–6; children aged 8–11 years) were invited to take part in the study.
Trial 2 inclusion criteria
All non-fee-paying primary schools within four London boroughs (Lewisham, Lambeth, Merton and Newham) with classes in Key Stage 2 (Years 3–6; children aged 8–11 years) were invited to take part in the study.
Exclusion criteria for trials 1 and 2
Independent schools, special schools, schools without all four year groups in Key Stage 2 at primary school (Years 3–6) and small schools with fewer than 15 pupils per year group were excluded.
Proposed sample size
Based on our previous school-based trial, Project Tomato,65 the SD for daily consumption in this age group was estimated to be 85 g for vegetables and 143 g for fruit, with an associated intraclass correlation coefficient of 0.125 for vegetables and 0.114 for fruit. With the proposed sample of one Year 3 class and one Year 4 class from each school, the sample size needed to detect a half-portion (40 g) difference in vegetable intake with 90% power would be 627 children per group, approximately 13 schools. 82 To have 90% power to detect a one-portion difference in fruit intake (one portion = 80 g), 482 children per group would be required, i.e. about 10 schools.
The Project Tomato research identified that approximately 75% of participants completed the dietary questionnaire at baseline and follow-up; therefore, to allow for possible withdrawals and children changing schools, it was decided that 16 schools would be randomly allocated to each group, except for the RHS-led intervention, where the sample size requirements were determined by the staffing levels at the RHS. As a consequence, the RHS-led group had a sample size of 10 randomised schools only, which was carried out by the trial research team.
Discontinuation criteria
Analysis followed the principle of intention to treat. Therefore all schools and children are included in the analysis according to the group to which they were initially randomised. All reasonable and ethical steps were taken to ensure completeness of follow-up of outcome measures.
School withdrawal
If a school wished to withdraw from the trial, the study team would post a data collection form to the head/class teacher along with a freepost envelope. The data collection form would record the following: reasons for withdrawal; whether or not anything could have been done to make taking part in the study easier; confirmation that they no longer wanted to take part in the intervention and receive information/training/materials; and whether or not they still allowed us to use data collected to date and to collect data at round two (i.e. follow-up collection) in October 2011.
Child withdrawal
This occurred if a parent requested to remove their child from the trial. It was anticipated that this request would go to the school, the RHS or the study team at the University of Leeds. Whoever was the first point of contact with the parent was required to inform the other relevant groups (school/RHS/University of Leeds) by telephone, letter or e-mail. A record of any child withdrawals was kept in the database. On receipt of this information, the study team would send a letter to inform the class teacher that the child was to be withdrawn from the study. A data collection form and freepost envelope would be sent via the class teacher to the parent. A covering letter would make it clear to the parent that although the child would not receive any self-study or home-based materials, he or she would not be left out of whole-class activities, as to do so would involve taking the child out of the class while these activities were occurring. The parent would be asked to complete the data collection form and post it back to the Nutritional Epidemiology Group at the University of Leeds in the freepost envelope.
Assessment of harm
On rare occasions, children or schools may need to discontinue the randomised intervention. This may, in most cases, be only a temporary withdrawal; for example, if a child injures him or herself with a spade. Minor adverse reactions were not considered grounds for discontinuing. However, these events were captured either by the RHS regional advisor for the RHS-led schools, or by the Nutritional Epidemiology Group team, through the process measures e-mail, for the teacher-led schools. All adverse events were reported to the Trial Steering Committee. The same notification procedures applied for school or individual withdrawal.
Interim analysis and stopping rules
No interim analyses of trial outcomes were planned.
Randomisation
Cluster randomisation, with school location and borough to identify each ‘cluster’, was used to randomise the schools. The schools were randomised by the study team by geographic location of their London borough and using Stata. From each primary school, one Year 3 class and one Year 4 class was asked to consent to be part of the trial. These classes were randomly selected if there was more than one class in that particular year group.
General considerations
All data collected from these two trials have been reported and presented according to the revised CONSORT statement in Chapter 6. 83
Ethical approval
Ethical approval was obtained through the University of Leeds Research Ethics Committee in 2009. Written informed consent was obtained first from all schools and then from all parents whose children were in the classes chosen to participate in the trial data collection. Schools and parents were informed about the potential risks and benefits of participating in the trial through the information sheet. Participants’ parents gave informed consent, with the opportunity to ‘opt out’ of the study if they did not wish their child to take part. If the parents did not wish their child to participate in the study, the child was still able to take part in the growing activities in the class; however, his or her food intake and child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire were not recorded.
The intervention: the Royal Horticultural Society Campaign for School Gardening
Intervention definitions
-
RHS-led intervention These schools received an intervention delivered by the RHS regional advisor.
-
Teacher-led intervention Staff from these schools attended twilight sessions of the garden programme at a nearby participating school. The twilight sessions were run by the RHS regional advisor.
The Campaign for School Gardening aims to:
-
inspire and empower schools to get growing and to give children the chance to grow and create gardens
-
demonstrate the value of gardening in enriching the curriculum, teaching life skills and contributing to children’s mental and physical health
-
convince everyone involved with education in schools of the value of gardening in developing active citizens and carers for the environment
-
understand the importance of plants and show how gardening can contribute to a sustainable environment.
The Royal Horticultural Society-led intervention
The RHS Campaign for School Gardening consisted of two programmes. The RHS-led intervention schools received the following:
-
a day visit from the RHS regional advisor each half-term to work in the garden with teachers and children (summer term 2010 to summer term 2011 inclusive)
-
follow-up visits to aid planning by the teachers who were leading the gardening activity (autumn term 2011 to autumn term 2012)
-
general ongoing advice on the school garden, and free seeds and tools
-
one twilight teacher training session each term (summer term 2010 to summer term 2011 inclusive), based on seasonal tasks in the school garden (open to RHS-led schools’ teachers and others from local schools)
-
free access to a wide range of teacher resources at www.rhs.org.uk/schoolgardening/.
The role of the regional advisor was to assist the schools in developing a successful garden, through working directly with teachers and pupils to give them support and practical advice (Figure 5). They were also expected to help overcome barriers to developing gardening within schools. The regional advisor had the expertise and experience to tie in gardening and growing activities with the national curriculum and to run staff training sessions for teachers. The key tasks of the regional advisor were to:
-
deliver advice and support to schools in setting up school gardens and growing projects
-
promote the RHS Campaign for School Gardening by contacting schools, local education authorities and partner organisations and by giving talks and demonstrations
-
train teachers in practical skills to grow plants and harvest crops
-
build community links and recruit volunteers to enable the wider community to support and get involved in school growing projects
-
contact, advise and support schools within the region by means of visits, e-mails and phone calls
-
make links with partner organisations and recruit volunteers to support schools in setting up school gardens and growing plants
-
run termly twilight training session courses at 10 school venues throughout the year.
An example of some of the work conducted in one of the RHS-led schools is described below.
-
Embedding gardening in the school in order to attain all the benefits which that brings (e.g. most pupils never have access to gardening, as they do not have gardens themselves).
-
Establishing a community garden which helps to deal with some of the difficult issues faced in the ‘forgotten estate’.
-
Redeveloping the school garden (to be used for class growing).
-
Creating simple beds, paths, a fence, and later possibly a greenhouse.
-
Digging a pit for the nursery to prevent the raised bed being ‘dug’.
-
Clearing the community allotment garden (‘secret garden’). The community garden is to be used for project work, teaching (e.g. about life cycles in a wildlife area) and community beds, and for use by learning mentors to work with children who have learning difficulties and/or behavioural issues.
The two images in Figure 6 below demonstrate the before-and-after effect of the RHS-led intervention in one of the 10 RHS-led schools.
The teacher-led intervention (‘teacher-led schools’)
The teacher-led intervention schools worked with the RHS by attending termly twilight training at a nearby RHS-led school, to help support them in developing and using their school gardens. Unlike the RHS-led schools, the teacher-led schools did not have direct support from the regional advisor. The regional advisor ran these twilight sessions for them, and provided the teacher-led schools with advice as needed for their school gardens. The following is an example of some of the topics taught in the twilight sessions.
Summer term 2010
-
Planning your school garden.
-
What and when to grow for the school term.
-
Watering in the school garden.
-
Introduction to garden pests.
Autumn term 2010
-
Garden site assessment and plans.
-
Bulb planting (including practical session, with free bulbs supplied).
-
School garden risk assessment templates.
-
Soil types and texture.
Spring term 2011
-
Safe tool use.
-
Seed sowing.
-
Growing for the school years.
-
Composting.
Summer term 2011
-
Watering.
-
Pricking out.
-
Garden tours.
Trial 1 consisted of schools participating in both intervention groups mentioned above, whereas for trial 2 schools were involved in either the teacher-led intervention or a comparison group. The comparison group did not receive any support from the regional advisor during the period of the trial. However, these schools were able to receive the twilight sessions for the summer of 2012, once the study had completed follow-up data collection.
Data collection methods
Data sources
The data used in this study came from the following sources.
Child-level data
-
School food diary, April 2010.
-
Home food diary, April 2010.
-
Child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire, April 2010.
School-level data
-
School gardening level questionnaire, June 2010.
-
Gardening in schools – process measures e-mail dated October 2010.
-
Information collated from the RHS advisor on school gardening in the intervention schools.
The main outcome measurements were collected at baseline in May and June 2010, when the children were in Years 3 and 4 (aged 7–8 years). The follow-up measurements were collected between October 2011 and January 2012, when the children were in Years 5 and 6 (aged 9–11 years). A breakdown of the different phases of these two trials is illustrated in Figure 7.
Training the fieldworkers: nutrition students
The primary schools were spread throughout London, and therefore a large sample of undergraduate or masters nutrition students were recruited to undertake baseline collection. These sessions were designed and led by MSC with assistance from one of the research assistants on the trial. The students were recruited from King’s College London and Roehampton University. The students were offered £70 payment per school, and were informed that in order to participate it would be necessary for them to attend one of the two training sessions offered in London. The first training session was at Roehampton University on 9 April 2010; the second was at King’s College London on 12 April 2010. Baseline collection took place from mid-April to July 2010. The students were not informed as to which intervention group the schools they visited were allocated.
Most of the students who registered an interest in the study were dietetic students, who had little data collection experience. In order to ensure that the standard of data collected was consistently high, training was provided to the students to teach them how to complete not only the school and home food diaries, but the child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire as well.
An important quality needed to work with children is presentation skills, the ability to speak confidently in a room full of young children. To assess the students on their ability to complete the baseline collection, the first part of the training required them to introduce themselves and explain how to play one of their favourite childhood games.
The next component of the training was a presentation by MSC introducing the students to the study, and what exactly their tasks would be if they were involved in the data collection. This was the first time the students had seen the questionnaires, so each section was explained to them in detail to help them familiarise themselves with the questionnaires. They were also shown the instructional DVD. The main part of the training consisted of two activities which are explained in detail below.
Sample diet exercise
This exercise involved giving the students examples of children’s food intake for the whole day. The aim was for the students to correctly code each food and categorise it in the right section of either the school or home food diary. An example of a child’s diet is presented in Table 9, shown with the correct school food diary codes. There were always some challenging food items included, which were typical for children to eat but not adults, such as the Dairylea Lunchables and Dunkers ham wrap.
Breakfast/before school | Morning break | Lunchtime | Before tea/after school | Evening meal/tea | After tea/during night |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
White bread toasted (C1)a with Utterly Butterly® (D1) Glass of apple juice (A6) |
Fruit and nut bar (B3) | Tropical-flavoured spring water (A4) Cheese and onion crisps (B1) Dairylea Lunchables – ham (E2, B5, G5) |
Satsuma (M5) | Chicken nuggets (F2) Chips (L2) Tomato ketchup (D2) Salt Vinegar |
Jaffa cakes (N5) White bread toasted (C1) with Utterly Butterly® (D1) |
Right or wrong
In the second activity, the students were presented with 10 completed home food diaries and were asked to identify whether the diaries had been completed by the parents correctly or incorrectly. The aim of this exercise was to show the students what to expect on day 2 of the baseline collection, and to identify when it is necessary to take a recall from a child due to serious errors in completion of the home food diary.
At the end of the session, the students had the opportunity to ask questions and raise queries about completing the different questionnaires and the overall structure of the data collection process.
Baseline collection
Baseline collection of the school and home food diaries, child questionnaire and school gardening telephone interview took place between April and July 2010. The baseline process measures e-mails were sent out in November 2010 followed by reminders in December 2010.
The sample consisted of 52 schools with a possibility of up to 2731 children being surveyed. The actual number of children that participated in the baseline collection was 1163 for trial 1 and 1417 for trial 2, giving a total of 2580 children before data cleaning, with 2529 children providing complete data for analysis. Two schools withdrew from the study, one because of teaching problems and another over concerns about Criminal Records Bureau checks, despite the fact that the students who were assigned to visit this school had been checked. The duration of the baseline collection was longer than anticipated owing to a volcanic eruption in Iceland delaying flights during the Easter break, leaving many schools understaffed. The undergraduate students trained to collect the data were efficient, though a small number withdrew from collecting data from a school at late notice. To prevent this occurring at follow-up the students were asked to sign a contract outlining their expectations in writing.
Follow-up collection
Follow-up collection of the school and home food diaries, child questionnaire and school gardening telephone interview took place from October 2011 to January 2012. The same process for recruiting students as that used at baseline was conducted for follow-up collection. All students who participated in follow-up collection attended a training session.
The follow-up process measures e-mails were sent out in December 2011 and a reminder was sent in January 2012. A number of the students who participated in baseline collection also participated in follow-up.
Data handling
Blinding
The project statistician (CE) allocated a random code for the different intervention groups and the control group involved in both trials. This was done to blind MSC to the intervention allocation while she was conducting the data cleaning and initial primary analysis, to ensure that there was no bias in the data cleaning method. Once the primary analysis was completed, the project statistician informed MSC of the code, so that she could finalise the secondary outcomes and final results. The details of school allocation for both trials was sealed in an envelope and kept in the principal investigator’s office.
Food and nutrient data
Data from baseline and follow-up school and home food diaries, based on CADET, were entered by Swift Research Ltd. The dietary information in the diaries was converted to a Microsoft Access spreadsheet providing the number of portions of 95 food types consumed at each of seven possible meal/snack events (breakfast/before school, morning break, lunchtime, afternoon break, before tea/after school, evening meal/tea and after tea/during night). For example, on the diary a child could tick sugary cereals at breakfast time. The database manager used predetermined age-related portion sizes to estimate the weight of all food types consumed. The database manger then used established in-house software named DANTE, based on the composition of foods67 and using standard predefined algorithms, to convert weights of foods into total daily nutrient values for each child. The 42 nutrients included total energy intake, macronutrients, vitamins and minerals, of which only those associated with fruit and vegetable intake were analysed further. These included total energy, fats, sugars, carbohydrates, fibre (non-starch polysaccharides), carotene, vitamin C, folate, zinc and iron. The 115 food types were reduced further to 14 categories, one of which was fruit (group M) and one of which was vegetables excluding potato (group L). Fruit juice was categorised as one category of group A (drinks). The weights of all types of fruit were summed to give the total weight for fruit, in addition to the total number of portions of fruit (one portion = 80 g). The weights of all types of vegetables were summed to give the total weight for vegetables, in addition to the total number of portions of vegetables.
Each child was given a unique identification code containing information on the school and the child. Follow-up and baseline data were combined using the unique identification codes for the children; therefore, no names or identifying information were included. The database was password protected so that only the database manager, project assistant and administrator and MSC could access the data. Any Microsoft Excel spreadsheets (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, WA, USA) with children’s names included (these were needed to identify the children for follow-up collection) also contained a password. Only MSC, the project assistant and administrator had access to this password.
Data cleaning
Values for non-dietary data collected at the follow-up phase were checked to ensure that all values were within plausible predetermined ranges. Out-of-range values were checked against original data to identify data entry errors. Errors due to data collection methods were recorded as missing.
Baseline and follow-up data were checked for completeness. Missing data for participants, such as date of birth and gender, were obtained from schools, where possible, by the project administrator. If these details were not available, children who had missing age data were given the mean age of children in their year group (Year 3, 4, 5 or 6). Where gender data were missing, they were given mean portion sizes, based on an average of boys and girls for that particular age group. Where both age and gender data were missing, then both steps above were applied.
The school and home food diaries were entirely tick box-based and were scanned; therefore, they were free of data entry errors. However, it was possible that there were scanning errors, such as diaries scanned the wrong way round or not lined up properly, or random marks mistaken for ticked boxes. Accurate scanning of diaries was initially checked by Swift Research Ltd. On arrival at the Nutritional Epidemiology Group, a random sample of the scanned diaries (approximately 10% of home diaries and 10% of school diaries) was inspected by MSC to provide a further check that the scanning process was accurate. Based on previous research into children’s diet diaries that have mean energy and/or total fruit and vegetable intake, ± three times the SD were identified as outliers and excluded.
Also, it was noticed from inspecting the baseline data that when a child ate fruit salad, several other types of fruit (more than three) were also ticked for that particular mealtime. It was decided to clean this data so that only fruit salad was recorded, as the fruit intake for that particular meal was considered too high for the majority of children.
Summary
This chapter has described the general methodological aspects that apply to both trials. It has explored how the schools were recruited and randomised, identified when the different data were collected, described the interventions and outlined the methods used to collate both baseline and follow-up data. Further descriptions of the statistical analysis will be described in detail in the relevant chapters exploring the results.
Chapter 4 Baseline food and nutrient characteristics
This chapter will explore the nutrient and food data from the home and school food diaries for all children in trials 1 and 2 combined. It will also explore children’s fruit and vegetable intake broken down by meal event and lunch type (packed or school meal) and the differences between boys and girls for key nutrients and food. In addition, it will explore how the home food environment and parental attitudes and values affect children’s fruit and vegetable intake.
Regression analysis
Linear regression analysis
Linear regression analysis explores the dependency of one variable – in this case, total fruit and vegetables consumed – on one or more other variables, such as gender, by fitting a linear equation to the observed data. Although the fundamental principles of regression remain the same, owing to the cluster randomisation of participants by school, multilevel regression methodology was applied to all statistical analyses in this chapter. 84
Clustered multilevel regression analysis
Multilevel regression analysis is often used for education-based data as it takes into consideration the hierarchical structure of school data. 85 In this study, level one is the individual child and level two is the school. Level 1, the individual level, is considered to be nested within the higher level, i.e. the school. It is based on the theory that all children’s food consumption within a school is similar; for example, children who eat a school meal will all have the same options or choice on any given day at that particular school, and are therefore more likely to consume similar foods. The benefit of this technique is that the means and confidence intervals (CIs) for the different foods and nutrients will be more accurate, if there is variation at school level. As children within a school have more similar food consumption to each other, there will be less variability in the sample from each school compared with a random sample from the whole population. 86 Also, multilevel modelling is not focused on the individual schools within the sample, but on estimating the patterns of variation within the population of schools. 86 If a single-level model was used instead for this analysis, ignoring the hierarchical structures within the data, this would lead to inaccurate or misleading results. The CIs would be too narrow, potentially leading to different conclusions. 86
Methods
Study population
This study includes baseline measurements from the children in both trials. These were children from 52 primary schools in the following London boroughs: Wandsworth, Tower Hamlets, Greenwich, Sutton, Lewisham, Lambeth, Merton and Newham.
Variables
The descriptive analysis uses results from the CADET school and home food diaries to describe food and nutrient intakes.
Further analysis used questions in section 2 of the home food diary, which asked about the child’s fruit and vegetable intake and the home environment. The responses were completed by the parent or carer. These questions explored fruit and vegetable habits in the family home:
-
Do you have different kinds of fruit/vegetables at home?
-
Do you buy a specific fruit/vegetable because your child asks for it?
-
Do you cut up fruit/vegetables for your child to eat?
-
Do you (parents) eat fruit/vegetables every day?
-
Do you eat fruit/vegetables together with your child?
-
Do you have to ask your child to eat their fruit or vegetables?
-
Do you allow your child to eat as much fruit/vegetables as she/he likes?
The responses to these questions were collected as ‘yes/always’, ‘yes most days/often’, ’sometimes’, ‘rarely’ and ‘never’. General summary statistics, including box plots and histograms of the different categories, were first analysed to identify the best method of coding the data. Based on the frequency of responses to these questions, they were then categorised ‘never/rarely’, ’sometimes’, or ‘always‘.
Four questions were designed to identify the factors associated with consumption habits of the family:72
-
the money I have available to spend on fruit and vegetables
-
the price of fruit and vegetables
-
the time I have available to prepare fruit and vegetables
-
likes and dislikes of my family for fruit and vegetables.
The responses to these questions were collected as ‘very important‘, ‘important‘, ‘neither important or unimportant‘, ‘unimportant’ and ‘very unimportant‘. Correlation tests indicated that these questions were highly correlated. These were recoded into a scale of 1 (not important at all) to 5 (very important).
In addition to these questions, the home inventory question ‘Please tick if you have any of the following fruit or vegetables in your fridge/freezer or cupboards’ was collected to identify the variety of fruit and vegetables in the home. The question ‘How many nights a week does your family eat together at a table?’ was asked to explore how the family meal habit affects children’s fruit and vegetable intake. As the response to this question can only be 0–7 it is considered a multinomial variable; therefore, it cannot be treated as a continuous variable. Total fruit and vegetable intake by the eight possible responses was explored. Owing to the similarity in total fruit and vegetable intake, in grams, for people who ate together at a table 1–6 nights a week, the data were recoded into the following: ‘never’ (0 nights a week), ’sometimes’ (1–6 nights a week) and ‘always’ (7 nights a week).
Statistical analysis
All statistical analysis was performed using Stata version 12. The descriptive statistics were performed for all key nutrients, foods, fruit and vegetables by meal event and demographic characteristics. An additional variable, based on the NHS 5 A DAY guidelines (www.nhs.uk/Livewell/5ADAY/Pages/5ADAYhome.aspx), was created to explore how many children were achieving the UK government’s fruit and vegetable target. This variable included all fruit and vegetables consumed, plus one portion (80 g) if pure fruit juice was consumed and one portion (80 g) if baked beans were consumed.
Analysis was then performed using clustered multilevel regression models to explore the differences between boys and girls for nutrients and food items. These models were first conducted unadjusted, and then adjusted for ethnicity and Index of Multiple Deprivation score (IMDS). The IMDS is a weighted measure based on the following categories: education, income, employment, health and/or disability, barriers to housing and services, crime and living environment. Where possible, the pupils’ postcodes were used to generate the IMDS; however, for those whose postcode was missing the school postcode was used. The output generated for the primary analysis was effect size, SD, 95% CIs and p-values, with a p-value of < 0.05% taken to represent statistical significance for all of the analyses. Mean values are presented in the tables, in some instances rounding has occurred when differences are referred to in the text. The same statistical methodology was applied to explore how home environment habits affect children’s mean nutrient intake.
The model fit was assessed by checking skewness and kurtosis (sktest), and q-normal probability plots and residuals. The sktest explores the skewness and kurtosis of the variables against the null hypothesis that the variable is normally distributed. 87 The skewness and kurtosis statistics describe the shape of the distribution. A score of 3 for the kurtosis statistic indicates that the variable is normally distributed, < 3 indicates that the distribution is flatter than a normal distribution and > 3 indicates that the distribution is higher pitched than a normal distribution. A symmetrical distribution should have a skewness of zero.
Results of baseline food and nutrient intakes
Basic characteristics
A total of 2529 children were asked to participate in the study, from 52 schools. After excluding school withdrawal and parents who did not consent, 2420 children received the intervention. After excluding children who did not complete both the home and school food diaries or who had a total energy and/or total fruit and vegetable intake more than three times the SD of the mean, the final sample size for baseline analysis was 2393, and the response rate was 94%. The mean age of the children (1188 girls and 1205 boys) was 8.3 years (95% CI 8.2 to 8.3 years). Of all the children in the sample, 29% received free school meals and 33% ate a packed lunch. English was spoken as an additional language by 46%, while 59% of children had a member of the family educated to degree level or higher. These results are presented in Table 10.
Baseline characteristics | n (%) |
---|---|
Boys | 1205 (50) |
Received free school meals | 693 (29) |
Ate packed lunch | 781 (33) |
Spoke EAL | 1147 (48) |
Family member with degree | 1410 (59) |
Ethnicity | |
White | 575 (24) |
Mixed | 200 (8) |
Asian or British Asian | 317 (13) |
Black or black British | 419 (18) |
Chinese or other ethnic group | 72 (3) |
Prefer not to say | 810 (34) |
Children’s nutrient intake
The mean, standard error (SE) and 95% CI for key nutrient intakes for the whole sample are presented in Table 11. The only nutrient not above the recommended mean was vitamin A, which was 100 µg lower than the recommended intake (mean 406 µg, 95% CI 388 to 424 µg). The mean energy intake for all children was 2018 kcal (95% CI 1990 to 2047 kcal). Total fat was 13 g higher than the recommended intake (mean 81 g, 95% CI 79 to 83 g), and sodium was 1508 mg higher than the recommended intake for this age group (mean 2658 mg, 95% CI 2604 to 2711 mg).
Nutrient | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Estimated average requirement/reference nutrient intake (age 7–10 years)a | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Girls | Boys | ||||
Energy (kcal) | 2018 | 15 | 1990 to 2047 | 1740 | 1970 |
Energy (KJ) | 8488 | 61 | 8369 to 8608 | 7280 | 8245 |
Protein (g) | 73 | 0.6 | 72 to 74 | 28 | 28 |
Carbohydrate (g) | 264 | 1.7 | 260 to 267 | 265 | 322 |
Fibre (Englyst) (g) | 12 | 0.1 | 12 to 12 | 18 | 18 |
Fat (g) | 81 | 0.8 | 79 to 83 | 68 | 77 |
Total sugars (g) | 130 | 1.0 | 128 to 132 | 123 | 140 |
Iron (mg) | 11 | 0.1 | 10 to 11 | 8.7 | 8.7 |
Calcium (mg) | 853 | 7.7 | 838 to 868 | 550 | 550 |
Potassium (mg) | 2727 | 20.3 | 2687 to 2767 | 2000 | 2000 |
Sodium (mg) | 2658 | 27.3 | 2604 to 2711 | 1200 | 1200 |
Folate (µg) | 226 | 1.9 | 222 to 230 | 150 | 150 |
Carotene (µg) | 2077 | 35 | 2007 to 2146 | 1700 | 1700 |
Vitamin A (retinol equivalent) (µg) | 406 | 9.3 | 388 to 424 | 500 | 500 |
Vitamin C (mg) | 111 | 1.4 | 108 to 114 | 30 | 30 |
Children’s key food and drink intake
The mean, SE and 95% CI for key foods for the whole sample are presented in Table 12. On average, children consumed 94 g of vegetables and 200 g of fruit, with a combined mean of 295 g of fruit and vegetables at baseline. Table 12 also shows the number (%) of children who consumed different foods and the mean intake of this subsample. From this analysis it is evident that 84% of the sample consumed some vegetables on the day of collection and 80% consumed some fruit, with 95% of the children eating either fruit or vegetables. The other most commonly consumed items were drinks; fizzy pop/squash was consumed by 53%, fruit juice by 51% and milk by 43% of the sample.
Food | Total sample (n = 2393) | Consumers only | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | n | % | Mean | SE | 95% CI | |
Total vegetables (excluding pulses, beans, lentils, dahl or seeds) (g) | 94 | 1.7 | 91 to 98 | 2006 | 84 | 113 | 1.7 | 109 to 116 |
Pulses, beans, seeds (g) | 16 | 0.8 | 14 to 17 | 455 | 19 | 85 | 2.4 | 73 to 86 |
Total fruit (g) | 200 | 3.5 | 193 to 206 | 1909 | 80 | 251 | 3.5 | 244 to 257 |
Fruit (non-dried) (g) | 199 | 3.4 | 192 to 206 | 1900 | 79 | 251 | 3.5 | 244 to 258 |
Dried fruit (g) | 2 | 0.2 | 1.3 to 2.0 | 103 | 4 | 38 | 1.7 | 35 to 41 |
Total fruit and vegetables (excluding pulses and beans) (g) | 295 | 4.1 | 286 to 303 | 2269 | 95 | 311 | 4.1 | 303 to 319 |
5 A DAY portions (80 g) | 4 | 0.1 | 4 to 4 | 2336 | 98 | 4 | 0.3 | 4 to 4 |
Sweets, toffees, mints (g) | 4 | 0.2 | 3 to 4 | 380 | 16 | 26 | 0.5 | 25 to 27 |
Chocolate bars (g) | 7 | 0.3 | 6 to 8 | 446 | 18 | 39 | 0.6 | 38 to 40 |
Crisps, savoury snacks (g) | 11 | 0.3 | 10 to 12 | 916 | 38 | 30 | 0.3 | 29 to 30 |
Nuts (g) | 1 | 0.1 | 1 to 2 | 93 | 4 | 37 | 1.6 | 34 to 40 |
Milk or milky drinks (ml) | 108 | 2.9 | 102 to 114 | 1028 | 43 | 253 | 3.6 | 146 to 260 |
Fizzy pop, squash, fruit drinks (ml) | 185 | 4.5 | 176 to 194 | 1259 | 53 | 352 | 5.2 | 342 to 362 |
Fruit juice (pure) (ml) | 123 | 3.0 | 117 to 129 | 1222 | 51 | 241 | 3.5 | 235 to 248 |
Fruit and vegetable intake by meal event
Further analysis was conducted to explore fruit and vegetable consumption by lunch type. These results are presented in Table 13 for the whole sample and for consumers only; 2269 children consumed fruit or vegetables during the day, meaning that only 124 children did not consume any at all. The most common times to consume fruit were lunch and before tea/after school, with the largest proportion of children, 38%, consuming fruit at lunchtime. Lunch was also one of the most common mealtimes to consume vegetables, with the largest proportion of children, 58%, consuming vegetables with their evening meal.
Type of lunch | Whole sample | Mean consumption: consumers only | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | Mean (g) | SE (g) | 95% CI (g) | n | % | Mean (g) | SE (g) | 95% CI (g) | |
Fruit intake | |||||||||
School meal | 1571 | 189 | 4.1 | 140 to 243 | 1396 | 58 | 243 | 4.2 | 234 to 251 |
Packed lunch | 772 | 231 | 6.4 | 218 to 243 | 567 | 24 | 267 | 6.3 | 255 to 280 |
Vegetable intake | |||||||||
School meal | 1571 | 106 | 2.1 | 102 to 110 | 1208 | 50 | 119 | 2.2 | 115 to 123 |
Packed lunch | 772 | 73 | 2.7 | 67 to 78 | 665 | 28 | 99 | 3.0 | 93 to 105 |
Difference in fruit and vegetables between packed lunches and school meals
At lunchtime, children can have either a school meal (provided by the school) or a packed lunch (provided by the parents). Table 13 displays the breakdown of fruit and vegetables based on lunch type. These results show that fruit intake was, on average, 42 g higher in children who had packed lunch meals compared with children who had school meals, and vegetable intake was 33 g higher in children who had school meals compared with children who had a packed lunch.
Differences in key nutrient intake between boys and girls
Multilevel regression analysis was conducted to explore the differences between boys and girls in this sample. Table 14 displays the means and SDs/SEs for boys and girls, and the unadjusted and adjusted regression results. These results identified that there is a significant difference between boys and girls for fibre, potassium, sodium, carotene and vitamin C, after adjusting for ethnicity and IMDS.
Nutrient | Girls (n = 1189) | Boys (n = 1205) | Unadjusted | Adjusted for ethnicity and IMDS | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean difference | SE | Mean difference | SE | 95% CI | p-value | |
Energy (kcal) | 2015 | 19 | 1977 to 2052 | 2023 | 22 | 1980 to 2066 | –8 | 30 | –13 | 28 | –70 to 43 | 0.6 |
Energy (KJ) | 8472 | 81 | 8314 to 8630 | 8506 | 91 | 8326 to 8685 | –34 | 129 | –55 | 118 | –292 to 182 | 0.6 |
Protein (g) | 73 | 0.8 | 72 to 75 | 74 | 1.0 | 72 to 76 | –1 | 1 | –1 | 1 | –4 to 2 | 0.3 |
Carbohydrate (g) | 265 | 2.4 | 261 to 270 | 263 | 2.4 | 258 to 268 | 2 | 4 | 1 | 5 | –6 to 8 | 0.8 |
Fibre (Englyst) (g) | 13 | 0.2 | 13 to 13 | 12 | 0.2 | 12 to 13 | 1 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 0 to 1 | < 0.001 |
Fat (g) | 81 | 1.0 | 79 to 83 | 82 | 1.4 | 80 to 85 | –1 | 2 | 1 | 2 | –4 to 8 | 0.3 |
Total sugars (g) | 132 | 1.4 | 129 to 134 | 130 | 1.5 | 127 to 133 | 2 | 2 | 1 | 2 | –4 to 5 | 0.7 |
Iron (mg) | 11 | 0.1 | 11 to 11 | 11 | 0.1 | 11 to 11 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 to 0 | 0.7 |
Calcium (mg) | 865 | 10.3 | 845 to 886 | 843 | 11.4 | 820 to 865 | 23 | 16 | 19 | 16 | –12 to 51 | 0.2 |
Potassium (mg) | 2809 | 29 | 2753 to 2864 | 2648 | 29 | 2591 to 2704 | 161 | 44 | 147 | 43 | 61 to 234 | 0.001 |
Sodium (mg) | 2592 | 30 | 2532 to 2651 | 2724 | 45 | 2636 to 2813 | –133 | 56 | –131 | 53 | –238 to –24 | 0.01 |
Folate (µg) | 228 | 2.5 | 223 to 233 | 225 | 2.9 | 219 to 230 | 3 | 4 | 2 | 4 | –6 to 11 | 0.5 |
Carotene (µg) | 2250 | 54 | 2153 to 2366 | 1898 | 45 | 1809 to 1986 | 352 | 89 | 345 | 90 | 164 to 526 | < 0.001 |
Vitamin A (retinol equivalent) (µg) | 389 | 10 | 368 to 409 | 424 | 15 | 394 to 453 | –35 | 18 | –29 | 17 | –63 to 4 | 0.08 |
Vitamin C (mg) | 119 | 2 | 116 to 123 | 104 | 1.9 | 100 to 107 | 16 | 3 | 15 | 3 | 8 to 20 | < 0.001 |
Differences in key food and drink intake between boys and girls
Further analysis was conducted only on boys and girls who consumed particular foods or drinks (Table 15). Girls, on average, consumed 20 g (95% CI 12 to 25 g) more vegetables, 14 g (95% CI 10 to 17 g) more dried fruit, 37 g (95% CI 20 to 54 g) more total fruit and vegetables (excluding pulses and beans), 19 g (95% CI 13 to 25 g) more nuts and 11 ml (95% CI –3 to 25 ml) more fruit juice. Boys, on average, consumed 5 g (95% CI 3 to 8 g) more sweets and 63 ml (95% CI 45 to 81 ml) more fizzy pop than girls.
Food | Girls | Boys | Unadjusted | Adjusted for ethnicity and IMDS | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean difference | SE | Mean difference | SE | 95% CI | p-value | |
Total vegetables (excluding pulses, beans, lentils, dahl and seeds) (g) | 105 | 2.5 | 100 to 110 | 85 | 2.3 | 80 to 89.4 | 20 | 4 | 20 | 4 | 12 to 25 | < 0.001 |
Pulses, beans, seeds (g) | 80 | 3.3 | 74 to 86.7 | 92 | 3.5 | 85 to 98.6 | –12 | 5 | –11 | 5 | –20 to –2 | 0.02 |
Total fruit (g) | 211 | 4.9 | 201 to 221 | 190 | 4.9 | 180 to 199 | 21 | 6 | 18 | 7 | 4 to 32 | 0.01 |
Fruit (non-dried) (g) | 254 | 4.9 | 244 to 263 | 249 | 5.1 | 239 to 258 | 5 | 5 | 3 | 6 | –9 to 15 | 0.6 |
Dried fruit (g) | 44 | 2.3 | 39 to 48 | 29 | 1.1 | 27 to 31 | 14 | 2 | 14 | 2 | 10 to 17 | < 0.001 |
Total fruit and vegetables (excluding pulses and beans) (g) | 316 | 5.8 | 305 to 327 | 274 | 5.8 | 263 to 286 | 41 | 8 | 37 | 9 | 20 to 54 | < 0.001 |
Number of 5 A DAY portions (80 g) | 4.6 | 0.08 | 4.5 to 4.8 | 4.1 | 0.08 | 3.9 to 4.3 | 0.5 | 0 | 0.5 | 0 | 0.2 to 0.7 | < 0.001 |
Sweets, toffees, mints (g) | 25 | 0.8 | 22 to 26 | 30 | 0.6 | 28 to 31 | –5 | 1 | –5 | 1 | –8 to –3 | < 0.001 |
Chocolate bars (g) | 38 | 0.9 | 38 to 41 | 39 | 0.8 | 37 to 41 | 0.9 | 1.0 | 1.1 | 1.1 | –1.0 to 3.3 | 0.3 |
Crisps, savoury snacks (g) | 31 | 0.5 | 30 to 32 | 29 | 0.4 | 29 to 30 | 2 | 1 | 2 | 1 | 1 to 3 | 0.004 |
Nuts (g) | 48 | 2.4 | 43 to 52 | 29 | 0.9 | 26 to 30 | 20 | 3 | 19 | 3 | 13 to 25 | < 0.001 |
Milk or milky drinks (ml) | 251 | 4.8 | 242 to 260 | 256 | 5.3 | 245 to 266 | –5 | 7 | –3 | 7 | –17 to 10 | 0.6 |
Fizzy pop, squash, fruit drinks (ml) | 318 | 6.4 | 305 to 330 | 382 | 7.7 | 366 to 397 | –63 | 10 | –63 | 9 | –81 to 45 | < 0.001 |
Fruit juice (pure) (ml) | 248 | 5.0 | 238 to 257 | 235 | 4.8 | 226 to 245 | 12 | 7 | 11 | 7 | –3 to 25 | 0.1 |
Differences in fruit and vegetable intake by meal event between boys and girls
The differences, by meal events, between boys and girls who consumed fruit and vegetables are presented in Table 16. On average, girls consumed 7 g (95% CI 3 to 11 g) more vegetables at lunchtime and 10 g (95% CI 4 to 15 g) more vegetables with their evening meal than boys, after adjusting for ethnicity and IMDS.
Mealtime | Girls | Boys | Unadjusted | Adjusted for ethnicity and IMDS | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n | Mean (g) | SE (g) | 95% CI (g) | n | Mean (g) | SE (g) | 95% CI (g) | Mean difference (g) | SE (g) | Mean difference (g) | SE (g) | 95% CI (g) | p-value | |
Fruit intake | ||||||||||||||
Breakfast/before school | 202 | 134 | 4.9 | 125 to 144 | 158 | 130 | 5.4 | 120 to 141 | 3.6 | 7.6 | 14 | 7.6 | 14 to 17 | 0.8 |
Morning break | 244 | 110 | 2.5 | 105 to 115 | 189 | 112 | 2.5 | 107 to 117 | –2.0 | 4.0 | –1.3 | 3.9 | –9 to 7 | 0.7 |
Lunchtime (all children) | 461 | 132 | 3.0 | 126 to 138 | 444 | 126 | 2.6 | 121 to 131 | 5.7 | 3.6 | 6.3 | 3.4 | –1 to 13 | 0.07 |
Afternoon break | 27 | 179 | 19.5 | 139 to 219 | 28 | 182 | 21.8 | 138 to 227 | –3.3 | 30.5 | –2.9 | 32.5 | –70 to 64 | 0.9 |
Before tea/after school | 415 | 145 | 4.3 | 136 to 154 | 366 | 155 | 4.9 | 145 to 165 | –9.6 | 5.5 | –10.9 | 5.6 | –22 to 1 | 0.05 |
Evening meal/tea | 264 | 133 | 4.7 | 124 to 142 | 259 | 136 | 4.6 | 127 to 145 | –3.4 | 7.0 | –2.5 | 7.2 | –16 to 51 | 0.7 |
After tea/during night | 278 | 136 | 4.5 | 127 to 144 | 239 | 141 | 5.2 | 131 to 152 | –5.6 | 6.4 | –7.5 | 6.5 | –20 to 5 | 0.2 |
Vegetable intake | ||||||||||||||
Breakfast/before school | 25 | 61 | 8.3 | 44 to 79 | 31 | 44 | 3.7 | 36 to 51 | 17.7 | 7.0 | 16.3 | 6.3 | 3 to 29 | 0.01 |
Morning break | 21 | 69 | 9.5 | 31 to 126 | 14 | 78 | 22.0 | 31 to 126 | –8.8 | 34.1 | –19.8 | 38.3 | –102 to 62 | 0.6 |
Lunchtime (all children) | 724 | 66 | 37.7 | 64 to 69 | 661 | 60 | 34.5 | 57 to 62 | 6.9 | 2.1 | 7.2 | 2.1 | 3 to 11 | 0.001 |
Afternoon break | 12 | 87 | 26.1 | 30 to 145 | 11 | 103 | 30.9 | 34 to 172 | –15.6 | 33.1 | –76.5 | 30.0 | –143 to –10 | 0.02 |
Before tea/after school | 141 | 61 | 3.4 | 55 to 68 | 124 | 64 | 5.1 | 54 to 74 | –2.2 | 6.5 | –1.6 | 6.5 | –14 to 11 | 0.8 |
Evening meal/tea | 736 | 83 | 2.0 | 80 to 87 | 655 | 74 | 2.0 | 70 to 78 | 9.5 | 2.6 | 9.5 | 2.8 | 4 to 15 | 0.001 |
After tea/during night | 64 | 62 | 5.5 | 51 to 74 | 59 | 57 | 3.8 | 50 to 64 | 5.3 | 5.6 | 5.4 | 5.7 | –6 to 17 | 0.01 |
Family meals can help children reach their 5 A DAY: further analysis of the baseline data
Epidemiological evidence indicates that a diet rich in fruit and vegetables can decrease the risk of developing cardiovascular disease, stroke, hypertension, type 2 diabetes mellitus, obesity and several forms of cancer. 3,4,91–94 A diet low in fruit and vegetables is one of the top 10 risk factors for global mortality. 16 Of particular public health concern is the rise of obesity in children. 95 In the UK, 1 in 10 children aged 2–10 years is obese. 10 Diet plays a fundamental role in weight management. Having a healthy diet rich in fruit and vegetables, which are low energy density foods, could potentially help tackle this epidemic. In the last 4 years the Department of Health has spent over £3.3M on the 5 A DAY campaign and £75M on the Change4Life campaign to rectify poor diets. 96 However, these campaigns do not directly address family mealtime behaviour. With the average child in the UK consuming less than the recommended intake of fruit and vegetables, it is important to identify influential factors associated with improving children’s overall nutrition.
There is evidence that dietary habits are developed in childhood and persist throughout life; therefore, it is vital that children at a young age consume adequate levels of fruit and vegetables. 6,7 Parents are the most influential factor in determining the quality of a child’s diet. 97,98 Parents’ attitudes and beliefs determine what food is offered to their children. Several studies have also indicated that children’s fruit and vegetable intake is positively associated with their parents’ intake. 8,99 Part of the influence that parents have on their children’s food intake is through modelling. Modelling is an important way for children to learn about eating; watching the way their parents eat and the different types of food they eat is pivotal in creating their own food habits and preferences. 59 Children need to see adults eating fruit and vegetables to help demonstrate positive behaviour. 100 However, there are few studies conducted in the UK that explore how the provision of fruit and vegetables in the home environment affects children’s overall intake. Using a large sample of children from London, this study aims to further explore and identify characteristics of the home food environment associated with children’s fruit and vegetable intake.
Children’s fruit and vegetable consumption and the home food environment
Clustered (by school) multilevel regression models, with total fruit and vegetable consumption as the primary outcome, were conducted to explore how the home food environment affects children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Table 17 displays the results, unadjusted and adjusted for children’s gender, ethnicity and IMDS.
Question | n | Unadjusted model | Adjusted model | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Fruit and vegetable amount (g) | p diff | p-trend | Fruit and vegetable amount (g) | 95% CI (g) | p diff | p-trend | ||
How often do you eat together as a family at a table? | ||||||||
Reference category: never | 92 | 1 | 1 | |||||
Sometimes | 768 | 96 | < 0.001 | 95 | 57 to 133 | < 0.001 | ||
Always | 656 | 126 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | 125 | 92 to 157 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 |
Do you cut up fruit and vegetables for your child to eat? | ||||||||
Reference category: never | 255 | 1 | 1 | |||||
Sometimes | 495 | 28 | 0.04 | 21 | –6 to 49 | 0.1 | ||
Always | 820 | 55 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | 44 | 18 to 71 | 0.001 | < 0.001 |
Do you eat fruit and vegetables together with your child? | ||||||||
Reference category: never | 109 | 1 | 1 | |||||
Sometimes | 439 | 8 | 0.7 | 10 | –36 to 57 | 0.6 | ||
Always | 1018 | 42 | 0.05 | < 0.001 | 39 | –2.5 to 80 | 0.04 | 0.03 |
Do you (parent/carer) eat fruit and vegetables every day? | ||||||||
Reference category: never | 58 | 1 | 1 | |||||
Sometimes | 258 | 48 | 0.1 | 43 | –14 to 99 | 0.1 | ||
Always | 1260 | 93 | < 0.001 | < 0.001 | 87 | 37 to 138 | 0.001 | < 0.001 |
Do you have different kinds of fruit and vegetables at home? | ||||||||
Reference category: never | 28 | 1 | 1 | |||||
Sometimes | 214 | 36 | 0.3 | 24 | –54 to 101 | 0.5 | ||
Always | 1368 | 75 | 0.03 | 0.01 | 66 | –2 to 135 | 0.05 | 0.01 |
Do you buy specific fruit and vegetables because your child asks for it? | ||||||||
Reference category: never | 166 | 1 | 1 | |||||
Sometimes | 542 | 21 | 0.3 | 15 | –24 to 53 | 0.4 | ||
Always | 873 | 27 | 0.1 | 0.3 | 20 | –17 to 57 | 0.2 | 0.5 |
Do you have to ask your child to eat their fruit and vegetables? | ||||||||
Reference category: never | 582 | 1 | 1 | |||||
Sometimes | 477 | –12 | 0.4 | –12 | –43 to 19 | 0.4 | ||
Always | 513 | –21 | 0.1 | 0.4 | –27 | –57 to 5 | 0.09 | 0.2 |
Do you allow your child to eat as much fruit and vegetables as they like? | ||||||||
Reference category: never | 78 | 1 | 1 | |||||
Sometimes | 180 | 12 | 0.6 | 5 | –52 to 62 | 0.8 | ||
Always | 1324 | 34 | 0.1 | 0.2 | 24 | –25 to 73 | 0.3 | 0.4 |
Mealtime behaviour
Children from families who reported ‘always’ eating a family meal together at a table consumed, on average, 125 g (95% CI 92 to 157 g) more fruit and vegetables than those from families who reported ‘never’ eating a meal together. Children from families who reported ’sometimes’ eating a family meal together ate, on average, 95 g (95% CI 57 to 133 g) more fruit and vegetables than those children who never ate a family meal together at a table.
Parental role modelling and fruit and vegetable consumption
The children of parents who eat fruit and vegetables every day consumed, on average, 87 g (95% CI 37 to 138 g) more fruit and vegetables than children whose parents never/rarely eat fruit and vegetables. Having different types of fruit and vegetables at home was also associated with increased fruit and vegetable intake. ‘Always’ having to ask a child to eat his or her fruit and vegetables had a non-significant inverse relationship with overall intake.
Provision of fruit and vegetables
Children whose parents always cut up fruit and vegetables for them consumed, on average, half a portion more (44 g, 95% CI 18 to 71 g), and those whose parents sometimes cut up fruit and vegetables an average of 21 g (95% CI –6 to 49 g) more, than the children of parents who never cut up their fruit and vegetables. There were no significant differences in fruit and vegetable consumption if parents bought specific fruit and vegetables for their children.
Clustered (by school) multilevel regression models, with total fruit and vegetable intake as the primary outcome, were conducted to explore the effect of the number of different types of fruit and vegetables that people had in their households on the questionnaire completion day. The results indicated that for every additional type of fruit or vegetable in the house, children’s fruit and vegetable intake increased by 5 g (95% CI 4 to 6 g, p < 0.001), after adjusting for sex, ethnicity and IMDS. Further analysis was conducted to explore whether or not there was an association with preparation time and cost of fruit and vegetables on a scale of 1 (unimportant) to 10 (very important). The models showed that there were no significant differences (preparation time: 3 g, 95% CI 0 to 6 g, p = 0.9; cost: 3 g, 95% CI –1 to 6 g, p = 0.9).
Children’s nutrient intake and key foods
Multilevel modelling was conducted to explore whether or not there was any difference in mean nutrient intake depending on family mealtime behaviour. These results are presented in Table 18. The results show that there was a significant difference in mean carbohydrates, fat, sugar, folate, carotene, vitamin C, fruit and vegetable intake and 5 A DAY portions, with higher intakes in families who reported always eating together. For families who reported always eating together at a table, children met the government recommendations for 5 A DAY (5.0 portions, 95% CI 4.8 to 5.2 portions), compared with families who reported sometimes eating together (4.6 portions, 95% CI 4.5 to 4.8 portions) and families who reported never eating together at a table (3.3 portions, 95% CI 2.8 to 3.8 portions).
Nutrient or food | Frequency of eating together as a family at a table | p-trenda | All children (n = 2389) | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Never (n = 92) | Sometimes (n = 768) | Always (n = 656) | |||||||||||
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | ||
Nutrient | |||||||||||||
Energy (kcal) | 1960 | 75.5 | 1810 to 2110 | 2078 | 25.9 | 2027 to 2129 | 2115 | 27.6 | 2061 to 2170 | 0.1 | 2019 | 14.5 | 1990 to 2047 |
Energy (KJ) | 8240 | 316.2 | 7612 to 8868 | 8740 | 108.5 | 8526 to 8953 | 8896 | 115.5 | 8669 to 9123 | 0.1 | 8489 | 60.8 | 8370 to 8608 |
Protein (g) | 72 | 3.9 | 65 to 80 | 75 | 1.1 | 73 to 77 | 77 | 1.2 | 75 to 80 | 0.1 | 74 | 0.6 | 72 to 75 |
Carbohydrate (g) | 250 | 7.5 | 235 to 265 | 273 | 3.0 | 267 to 278 | 279 | 3.2 | 273 to 285 | < 0.001 | 264 | 1.7 | 261 to 267 |
Fibre (Englyst) (g) | 11 | 0.5 | 10 to 12 | 13 | 0.2 | 12 to 14 | 14 | 0.2 | 13 to 15 | 0.6 | 13 | 0.1 | 12 to 13 |
Fat (g) | 82 | 4.6 | 73 to 91 | 84 | 1.5 | 81 to 87 | 85 | 1.6 | 82 to 88 | 0.01 | 82 | 0.8 | 80 to 83 |
Total sugars (g) | 120 | 5.0 | 110 to 130 | 137 | 1.9 | 133 to 141 | 140 | 2.0 | 136 to 144 | 0.01 | 131 | 1.0 | 129 to 133 |
Iron (mg) | 11 | 0.5 | 9 to 12 | 11 | 0.2 | 11 to 12 | 12 | 0.2 | 11 to 12 | 0.1 | 11 | 0.09 | 10 to 11 |
Folate (µg) | 217 | 10.6 | 196 to 238 | 236 | 3.5 | 229 to 243 | 246 | 3.7 | 238 to 253 | 0.02 | 226 | 2.0 | 222 to 230 |
Carotene (µg) | 1744 | 181.4 | 1384 to 2104 | 2139 | 63.2 | 2014 to 2263 | 2412 | 70.5 | 2274 to 2551 | < 0.001 | 2077 | 35.2 | 2008 to 2147 |
Vitamin C (mg) | 97 | 6.0 | 85 to 109 | 119 | 2.6 | 114 to 124 | 125 | 2.7 | 111 to 130 | < 0.001 | 111 | 1.4 | 109 to 114 |
Food | |||||||||||||
Total vegetables (excluding pulses, beans, lentils or seeds) (g) | 68 | 7.1 | 54 to 82 | 99 | 3.1 | 93 to 105 | 113 | 3.5 | 106 to 119 | < 0.001 | 95 | 1.7 | 92 to 98 |
Pulses, beans, seeds (g) | 15 | 4.0 | 7 to 23 | 17 | 1.3 | 14 to 19 | 20 | 1.8 | 17 to 24 | 0.2 | 86 | 2.4 | 81 to 91 |
Total fruit (g) | 148 | 14.7 | 119 to 177 | 213 | 6.4 | 200 to 226 | 229 | 6.8 | 216 to 242 | < 0.001 | 200 | 3.5 | 193 to 207 |
Dried fruit (g) | 35 | 8.4 | 7 to 61 | 41 | 2.8 | 36 to 47 | 36 | 2.6 | 31 to 41 | 0.5 | 39 | 17.2 | 35 to 42 |
Total fruit and vegetables (excluding pulses and beans): number of 80 g portions | 2.7 | 0.2 | 2.3 to 3.1 | 3.9 | 0.1 | 3.7 to 4.1 | 4.3 | 0.1 | 4.1 to 4.5 | < 0.001 | 3.7 | 0.1 | 3.6 to 3.8 |
Number of 5 A DAY portions (80 g) | 3.3 | 0.2 | 2.8 to 3.8 | 4.6 | 0.9 | 4.5 to 4.8 | 5.0 | 0.1 | 4.8 to 5.2 | < 0.001 | 4.3 | 0.1 | 4.3 to 4.5 |
Fruit juice, pure (ml) | 112 | 14.2 | 84 to 140 | 138 | 5.7 | 126 to 149.2 | 138 | 6.0 | 126 to 149 | 0.2 | 124 | 3.0 | 117 to 130 |
Discussion
Overall, the nutrient levels of all children in this sample were adequate when compared against the Department of Health recommendations,89 with children’s mean iron, folate and carotene levels all meeting recommended levels. 101 Children’s mean fat and sodium intakes were above the recommended levels. With rising rates of obesity in children,102 consumption of high energy density foods needs to be reduced. Diet plays a fundamental role in weight management; having a healthy diet consisting of high levels of low energy density foods could help tackle this epidemic. 28,103 Our results reflect those found in the 1999–2000 NDNS analysis,68 in which children’s fat and iron intakes were above the maximum requirements set by the Scientific Advisory Committee on Nutrition. 104
Overall energy levels were appropriate for children of this age group. These results identified that there were some differences between boys and girls for fibre, potassium, sodium, carotene and vitamin C, after adjusting for ethnicity and IMDS. A difference was also found in the types of food that boys and girls consumed. On average, girls consumed more vegetables, dried fruit and fruit juice than boys, whereas boys consumed, on average, more sweets and fizzy drinks. Furthermore, girls tended to consume more fruit and vegetables than boys in the lunchtime meal and in their evening meal. This difference remained significant after adjusting for ethnicity and IMDS. These gender differences in fruit and vegetable consumption have also been found in previous research conducted in the same age group. 105 With dietary patterns established in childhood tending to persist throughout adulthood,6,7 this pattern of girls consuming more fruit and vegetables can also be seen in a teenage population106 and in the adult population. 68 This difference in fruit and vegetable intake between boys and girls needs to be addressed in future public health interventions. More research should be conducted to try and identify ways of encouraging boys to consume more fruit and vegetables.
The second half of this chapter explored the association between primary school children’s fruit and vegetable intake and their home food environment. This is the first large survey of London children to explore this association. We found that eating a family meal together at a table had the biggest effect on children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Children in families who stated that they ate together every day had one and a half more portions of fruit and vegetables daily than those from families who reported never eating together at a table, after adjusting for possible confounders. The survey also found that sometimes eating at a table together increased children’s fruit and vegetable consumption by more than one portion. The importance of the family meal is supported by previous research in preschool children99 and primary school children. 77,107,108 The majority of literature conducted in this area is from the USA. 107–113 One study has explored this association in the UK;77 this was a relatively small study with only 102 participants. It does, however, support our findings here, reporting that frequency of family meals can increase children’s fruit and vegetable consumption.
Family mealtime behaviour not only affects fruit and vegetable intake, but may also be a predictor of the general quality of a child’s diet. 114 McIntosh et al. 113 explored mothers’ planning behaviour around cooking and their attitudes towards the family meal, identifying that mothers’ belief in the family meal determined the frequency of this behaviour. Also, mothers who have a higher belief in the importance of eating a meal together were more likely to be motivated to plan their food shopping around cooking for a family meal. These results are similar to those of Jones et al. ,98 who found that maternal intake was a predictor of children’s fruit and vegetable intake. The regularity of parents’ fruit and vegetable consumption and the availability of fruit and vegetables in the home99,115,116 are considered important predictors of children’s intake. 65,107 There has also been research in older children (aged 9–14 years) which found that eating a family meal together was inversely associated with obesity in children in the USA. 117
There are benefits other than improving the family’s nutritional status to having a family meal together. It provides conversational time for families,77 incentives to plan a meal107 and an ideal environment for parents to model appropriate mealtime behaviour. As dietary habits are established in childhood, the importance of the family meal needs to be promoted in public health campaigns such as the ‘Every Contact Counts’ campaign,118 raising health consciousness using brief interventions.
This research also supports previous studies on preschool-age children which found that parental intake is strongly associated with children’s intake. 119,120 The more frequently parents consumed fruit and vegetables, the higher the consumption by their children. Parents eating fruit and vegetables with their children was also associated with higher child consumption. The relationship between parental intake and child’s intake can be explained through behaviour modelling, and the child’s simple desire to imitate his or her parents. 59,112,113 Increased availability would increase children’s familiarisation with different fruits and vegetables, which is considered to be a key determinant of children’s consumption. 17,19 Availability of different types of fruit and vegetables in the home could simply be providing children with the visual cue to eat a piece of fruit or vegetable. 17,121 Future interventions could be tailored towards improving parental intake of fruit and vegetables, to facilitate their children’s intake.
Another important public health message, but one that is simple to implement, is that cutting up fruit and vegetables facilitates children’s intake. If children have access to prepared fruit and vegetables at home, they are more likely to eat them. Research supporting this finding has been conducted in older children. 101,122 This study is the first conducted in primary school children in the UK to support such findings.
The importance of a family eating together at a table becomes evident when exploring the differences between the key foods, with the mean fruit and vegetable intake for families who always eat an evening meal at a table reaching the government guidelines of five portions a day. 89 The 5 A DAY definition includes one portion of fruit juice and one portion of beans, as well as any fruit or vegetables consumed. One-third of the children in this sample report achieving this target. It is evident that eating a family meal together plays a vital role in improving children’s diets. There were also several macronutrients which were significantly higher in the families that always ate together at a table, such as folate, carotene, vitamin C and iron, all found in fruit and vegetables. Energy intake did not differ between families who ate together and those who did not; nevertheless, sugar and fat intakes were higher in those who ate together. However, the percentage of energy derived from fat was lower in those who always ate together (36% energy from fat) than in those who never ate together (38% energy from fat).
Strengths and limitations
There were some limitations of this study. There were 887 parents (36%) who did not complete the additional questions, and of these, 23% did not return the home food diary; therefore, the results are potentially subject to response bias. However, no differences were found when analysing with or without the missing participants. The CADET questionnaire was completed by trained fieldworkers in school hours, and by parents for the evening meal and breakfast. Parents and children might be inclined to give socially desirable responses, leading to an overestimation of the association between the home food environment and children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Reverse causation is also possible, in that children with good behaviour at mealtimes may encourage family eating, whereas fussy children might be left to eat alone. This type of dietary assessment has limitations; the portion size assumed for each item in CADET is based on weighed intakes from UK children. A 1-day tick list may not reflect true nutrient intake in the longer term.
Nevertheless, this study is particularly interesting as its population is from London, a highly diverse population in terms of ethnicity and socioeconomic groups. Response at baseline was high at 92%. Although the London boroughs chosen to sample were some of the more disadvantaged, we found that 46% of families spoke English as an additional language (EAL) and 59% had a family member educated to at least degree level. This is higher than the London average, with 55% of primary school children in London speaking EAL in 2012 and 38% of families including someone with a degree. 123 The responding sample may be more advantaged than the general London population. This could have influenced the results obtained, with higher intakes of fruit and vegetables than those found in the NDNS. 18
The dietary data were collected using the previously validated 24-hour food tick list, CADET. The strength of the CADET diary is that it uses age- and gender-specific food portion sizes to calculate food and nutrient intake. A 1-day tick list is an economically effective way of gathering nutrient information from children. Furthermore, all the results were conducted using multilevel analysis. The benefit of this technique is that the means and CIs for the different foods and nutrients will be more accurate; children within a school are more similar to each other in terms of their food consumption, with less variability within the sample compared with a random sample from the whole population. 86,124 In addition to previous research using this tool, a DVD with instructions for completing the questionnaire was sent home for parents/carers and children to watch, and a trained fieldworker reviewed the diary with the children to improve the home food data quality.
Conclusion
This analysis demonstrates a positive impact of the home environment on children’s fruit and vegetable intake. This could not only improve children’s dietary habits, but also those of parents. The key message from this research is for families to eat fruit and vegetables together at mealtimes. Cutting up fruit or vegetables for children facilitates their intake. Eating fruit and/or vegetables with children will increase their consumption, and could help them achieve the national recommendation. Successful public health interventions are needed to improve family food-related behaviour.
Summary
CADET found that children consumed, on average, 293 g (95% CI 287 to 303 g) of fruit and vegetables per day. The first half of this chapter described the energy and nutrient intake for all children from the RHS baseline collection. It also explored the differences between boys and girls in this sample. The second half of the chapter explored how the home environment affects children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Children of families who reported ‘always’ eating a family meal together at a table consumed 125 g more fruit and vegetables than those from families who never ate a meal together. Children of parents who consume fruit and vegetables daily had an intake 87 g higher than those whose parents rarely/never consume fruit and vegetables. Cutting up fruit and vegetables for children was associated with higher consumption. Families who reported always cutting up fruit and vegetables for their children consumed 44 g more fruit and vegetables than those who reported never cutting up fruit and vegetables. This chapter identified that cutting up fruit and vegetables and family meal consumption of fruit and vegetables facilitate children’s intake. Eating a family meal together regularly could increase children’s fruit and vegetable consumption and help them achieve the recommended intake.
Chapter 5 Evaluation of the impact of a school gardening intervention on children’s fruit and vegetable intake: results from two randomised controlled trials
Previous research into the impact of school gardening on children’s food intake has been hampered by variability in the quality of study design and the use of invalidated tools to measure children’s nutritional intake. This study used a robust methodology through two RCTs to explore how two different gardening interventions affect children’s fruit and vegetable consumption. This chapter addresses the primary outcome for both trials and the following aims for trials 1 and 2.
Primary outcome
-
Can the RHS Campaign for School Gardening lead to increases in vegetable and fruit intake in children aged 8–9 years?
The effectiveness of the RHS-led intervention compared with the teacher-led intervention (trial 1), or the teacher-led intervention compared with the comparison group (trial 2), would be determined by an increase in mean intake of fruit, vegetables, or fruit and vegetables at follow-up, after adjusting for baseline.
Secondary aims
-
What is the effect of the RHS-led intervention compared with the teacher-led intervention, or the teacher-led intervention compared with the comparison schools, on intake of key nutrients (fat, carbohydrate, protein, vitamin C, carotene, iron, sodium, folate)?
-
Is there an interaction between gender and the intervention?
Methodology
Details regarding the sampling methodology, ethics, data collection tools, randomisation, data cleaning and interventions are described in Chapter 3.
Study population
Trial 1 included 23 schools from the following London boroughs: Wandsworth, Tower Hamlets, Greenwich and Sutton. Trial 2 included 31 schools from the following London boroughs: Lewisham, Lambeth, Merton and Newham.
Statistical analysis
Variables
The primary objective of the trials was to evaluate the RHS Campaign for School Gardening by measuring the change in mean intake of daily portions of fruit and vegetables, daily portions of fruit and daily portions of vegetables, using data derived from the school and home food diaries.
-
All three variables are continuous and derived from the nutrient software dietary nutrition tool for evaluation (DANTE).
-
These measurements were taken at baseline (April 2010) and again at follow-up (15 months later).
Secondary aims measures
Nutrients
-
Total energy intake (MJ/day).
-
Fat intake (g/day).
-
Saturated fat intake (g/day).
-
Salt intake (g/day).
-
Sugars (g/day) including non-milk extrinsic sugars.
-
Carotene intake (mg/day).
-
Vitamin C intake (mg/day).
-
Vitamin D intake (mg/day).
-
Iron intake (μg/day).
-
Fibre intake (non-starch polysaccharides) (g/day).
-
Zinc intake (μg/day).
-
Carbohydrate intake (g/day).
-
Folate intake (μg/day).
Foods
-
Intake of foods that are high in fat, salt or sugar, and sugar-sweetened beverages.
General participant descriptive statistics and summary of primary and secondary outcomes/aims measures were tabulated for each intervention/control group within the two trials.
Comparison of intervention and control groups at baseline
School-level baseline characteristics were compared between groups for trials 1 and 2. This was performed to confirm that randomisation had resulted in broadly similar groups, in terms of weights of foods and nutrients and individual and school-level characteristics. Balance of school/class- and child-level variables between the two intervention groups was assessed using the following variables.
School/class level
-
Percentage of children with EAL.
-
Percentage of non-white children.
-
Percentage of children with free school meals eligibility.
Child level
-
Sex.
Primary outcome analysis of the trial
The variability between the schools determined which type of model should be used for this analysis. The main analysis used a cluster randomised regression random effects model, with change in total fruit and vegetables as the primary outcome to explore the study aims and objectives; results were reported both as unadjusted and adjusted for baseline intake. Analyses using random effects models were used to determine any differences between schools. This analysis was based on the theory of intention-to-treat (ITT) analysis, where all participants are analysed based on their randomised condition at baseline. The output that was generated for the primary analysis included effect size, SE, 95% CIs and p-values, with a p-value of < 0.05% taken to represent statistical significance. Mean values are presented in the tables, in some instances rounding has occurred when differences are referred to in the text.
Description of means of food types and nutrients by intervention status
In addition to comparison of baseline variables, the mean weight (g) of fruit and vegetables consumed on the follow-up CADET data collection day, with SE and 95% CIs, was recorded for all children. This was reported both with and without adjustment for baseline fruit and vegetable levels.
Secondary outcome analysis of the trial
Subgroups were compared by gender, including as an interaction term. A p-value of 0.01 was used to take into account multiple testing. These analyses answer plausibility questions, i.e. whether or not the intervention effect differs by gender.
Sensitivity analysis
This is an epidemiology-based RCT, and therefore it is typical that dropout would occur; approximately 25% of the baseline sample did not complete the trial. Reasons why participants dropped out were described in Chapter 4. Sensitivity analyses were carried out using baseline data brought forward to explore the effect on the primary outcome.
Results
Sample size
Ten schools were randomised to receive the RHS-led intervention and 13 schools to receive the teacher-led intervention in trial 1. In trial 2, 16 schools were allocated to receive the teacher-led and 15 to receive the comparison interventions. Our sample size at baseline for both trials (2529 children allocated to the intervention groups) was less than the original aim of 2900 children. The final sample size reduced to 1554, with only 641 children in total completing trial 1 (RHS-led: 312; teacher-led: 329); similar results were found in trial 2, with 916 children in total completing the trial (teacher-led: 488; control: 428). The response rate at follow-up for the two combined was 62%. This reduced the average group size to approximately 388, which was 94 children fewer than the proposed sample size of 482. This has reduced the power to detect the difference of one portion of fruit and vegetables from 90% to 83%.
The flow of schools and children through both trials is presented in the following four CONSORT diagrams (Figures 8–11).
Regression assumptions
The primary analysis for these trials explored fruit and vegetable intake using multilevel regression analysis, which requires the primary outcome to be broadly normally distributed and the residuals of the regression to be normally distributed. For children, fruit and vegetable intake is rarely normally distributed, as often a percentage of children do not consume any fruit or vegetables on a particular day. This leads to a negatively skewed distribution. Figure 12 shows the possible transformations that might improve the distribution of combined fruit and vegetable intake at follow-up. It is evident from the transformation options that none of these improve the general distribution of follow-up fruit and vegetable intake. Please note that the histogram labelled identity is the distribution without any transformation.
In addition to exploring the histogram of the distribution of follow-up fruit and vegetable intake, a plot of the residuals was explored to determine if it would be appropriate to use follow-up fruit and vegetable intake, adjusted for baseline fruit and vegetable intake, in the analysis. Figure 13 displays the plot of the residuals for follow-up fruit and vegetable intake from the primary multilevel regression analysis. From the figure it is evident that the distribution is skewed. Therefore, if the analysis was conducted using follow-up fruit and vegetable intake as the primary outcome, the regression assumptions would not be met.
In an attempt to better meet the regression assumptions, a change in the fruit and vegetable intake (follow-up intake minus baseline intake) variable was created. Figure 14 displays the histogram of the mean change in combined fruit and vegetable intake. It is evident from the histogram that the distribution of change in fruit and vegetable intake is much closer to a normal distribution than follow-up fruit and vegetable intake.
Further analysis of the residuals of mean change in combined fruit and vegetable intake is presented in Figure 15. The plot of the residuals illustrates that change in mean difference in fruit and vegetable intake is broadly normally distributed, making it suitable for multilevel regression analysis.
Change at follow-up has been used before to analyse RCTs. However, it is necessary to assess if there is a baseline imbalance between the two groups in these trials, to determine if it is appropriate to use change instead of adjusting for baseline. As there appeared to be little imbalance at baseline for fruit and vegetables in either trial, change in fruit and vegetable intake was used to analyse the primary outcome for both these trials.
General descriptive
Table 19 describes the demographic details for the children who completed trial 1. The children’s age (RHS-led mean 8.2 years, 95% CI 8.1 to 8.4 years; teacher-led mean 8.1 years, 95% CI 8.0 to 8.3 years), percentages of boys and girls and ethnicity were very similar between the two intervention groups. There was a difference in the percentage of children eligible for free school meals; in the RHS-led group, 33% received a free school meal, compared with 24% in the teacher-led group. The percentage of children with EAL also differed.
Characteristic | RHS-led (n = 312) | Teacher-led (n = 329) |
---|---|---|
Child | ||
Boys (%) | 50 | 51 |
Ethnicity, n (%) | ||
White | 92 (30) | 117 (35) |
Mixed | 18 (6) | 22 (7) |
Asian or British Asian | 72 (23) | 39 (12) |
Black or British black | 38 (12) | 55 (17) |
Chinese or other ethnic group | 10 (3) | 8 (2) |
Prefer not to say | 82 (26) | 88 (27) |
School | ||
FSME (%) | 33 | 24 |
IMDS | 34 | 30 |
Children with EAL (%) | 54 | 38 |
Table 20 describes the demographic details for the children who completed trial 2. The children’s age (comparison mean 8.2 years, 95% CI 8.2 to 8.3 years; teacher-led mean 8.3 years, 95% CI 8.2 to 8.3 years), percentages of boys and girls and ethnicity were very similar between the two groups. Again, the ethnic diversity of this sample is illustrated by trial 2. In trial 1 it was evident that there was a difference in free school meal eligibility between the two groups; however, for trial 2 there is very little difference in percentage receiving free school meals, IMDS and percentage of children with EAL.
Characteristic | Comparison group (n = 488) | Teacher-led (n = 428) |
---|---|---|
Child | ||
Boys (%) | 48 | 52 |
Ethnicity, n (%) | ||
White | 74 (17) | 111 (23) |
Mixed | 47 (11) | 42 (9) |
Asian or British Asian | 35 (8) | 68 (14) |
Black or British black | 85 (20) | 100 (20) |
Chinese or other ethnic group | 7 (2) | 21 (4) |
Prefer not to say | 177 (42) | 146 (30) |
School | ||
FSME (%) | 23 | 24 |
IMDS | 33 | 33 |
Children with EAL (%) | 42 | 47 |
Table 21 shows the baseline nutrient and food intake for all children in trial 1, broken down by intervention allocation (RHS-led and teacher-led). At baseline, values for key foods, nutrients and energy were all closely matched across the two intervention groups; the mean energy intake for the RHS-led group was 2085 kcal (95% CI 1971 to 2103 kcal) compared with the teacher-led mean intake of 2046 kcal (95% CI 1987 to 2103 kcal). There was only 5 g difference in mean carbohydrates intake (RHS-led mean: 265 g, 95% CI 257 to 272 g; teacher-led mean: 270 g, 95% CI 263 to 277 g); and 5 mg difference in vitamin C intake (RHS-led mean: 108 mg, 95% CI 102 to 114 mg; teacher-led mean: 103 mg, 95% CI 97 to 108 mg). There was a very small difference in fruit and vegetable intake, with the teacher-led group consuming on average more vegetables (RHS-led mean: 86 g, 95% CI 78 to 93 g; teacher-led mean: 101 g, 95% CI 94 to 106 g) and more total fruit (RHS-led mean: 190 g, 95% CI 174 to 204 g; teacher-led mean: 201 g, 95% CI 195 to 224 g).
Nutrient or food | RHS-led (n = 465) | Teacher-led (n = 563) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | |
Nutrient | ||||||
Energy (kcal) | 2038 | 33.4 | 1971 to 2103 | 2046 | 29.7 | 1987 to 2103 |
Energy (KJ) | 8568 | 140.0 | 8293 to 8843 | 8603 | 124.6 | 8358 to 2103 |
Protein (g) | 75 | 1.6 | 71 to 78 | 74 | 1.3 | 71 to 76 |
Carbohydrate (g) | 265 | 3.8 | 257 to 271 | 270 | 3.6 | 263 to 277 |
Fibre (Englyst) (g) | 13 | 0.3 | 12 to 13 | 14 | 0.2 | 13 to 14 |
Fat (g) | 83 | 1.9 | 79 to 86 | 82 | 1.6 | 78 to 84 |
Total sugars (g) | 130 | 2.4 | 125 to 135 | 132 | 2.2 | 127 to 136 |
Iron (mg) | 11 | 0.2 | 10 to 11 | 11 | 0.2 | 11 to 11 |
Calcium (mg) | 862 | 17.8 | 827 to 897 | 871 | 15.3 | 841 to 901 |
Potassium (mg) | 2778 | 47.4 | 2685 to 2871 | 2792 | 72.0 | 2709 to 2874 |
Sodium (mg) | 2686 | 66.4 | 2555 to 2816 | 2646 | 51.7 | 2544 to 2747 |
Folate (µg) | 228 | 4.5 | 218 to 236 | 226 | 3.9 | 218 to 233 |
Carotene (µg) | 1922 | 79.3 | 1766 to 2078 | 2249 | 75.8 | 2099 to 2397 |
Vitamin A (retinol equivalent) (µg) | 408 | 21.5 | 365 to 449 | 412 | 16.8 | 379 to 445 |
Vitamin C (mg) | 108 | 3.1 | 102 to 114 | 103 | 2.6 | 97 to 108 |
Food | ||||||
Total vegetables (excluding pulses, beans, lentils, dahl or seeds) (g) | 86 | 3.6 | 78 to 93 | 101 | 3.2 | 94 to 106 |
Pulses, beans, seeds (g) | 20 | 2.2 | 15 to 24 | 19 | 1.7 | 15 to 22 |
Fruit (non-dried) (g) | 190 | 7.6 | 174 to 204 | 210 | 7.3 | 195 to 224 |
Total fruit (g) | 192 | 7.7 | 176 to 206 | 208 | 7.3 | 193 to 222 |
Dried fruit (g) | 2 | 0.4 | 1 to 2 | 2 | 0.4 | 1 to 3 |
Total fruit and vegetables (excluding pulses and beans) (g) | 276 | 8.9 | 258 to 293 | 310 | 8.4 | 293 to 326 |
Sweets, toffees, mints (g) | 5 | 0.5 | 3 to 5 | 4 | 0.4 | 3 to 5 |
Chocolate bars (g) | 8 | 0.8 | 6 to 9 | 8 | 0.7 | 8 to 9 |
Crisps, savoury snacks (g) | 12 | 0.8 | 10 to 13 | 10 | 0.6 | 9 to 11 |
Nuts (g) | 1 | 0.4 | 0.5 to 2 | 1 | 0.3 | 0.6 to 1 |
Milk or milky drinks (ml) | 131 | 7.2 | 117 to 145 | 105 | 5.7 | 94 to 116 |
Fizzy pop, squash, fruit drinks (ml) | 166 | 9.4 | 147 to 184 | 167 | 8.8 | 150 to 184 |
Fruit juice (pure) (ml) | 122 | 7.0 | 108 to 135 | 104 | 5.5 | 93 to 114 |
Table 22 shows the baseline nutrient and food intake for all children in trial 2 broken down by intervention allocation (teacher-led and comparison group). At baseline, values for key foods, nutrients and energy were all closely matched across the two groups. Compared with trial 1, there was a small difference in mean energy intake between the two groups, with the teacher-led group consuming on average 2034 kcal (95% CI 1979 to 2089 kcal) and the comparison group consuming on average 1970 kcal (95% CI 1917 to 2021 kcal). There was a small difference of 11 g in mean carbohydrates intake (teacher-led mean: 267 g, 95% CI 260 to 273 g; comparison group mean: 256 g, 95% CI 250 to 262 g), and a 2 mg difference in vitamin C intake (teacher-led mean: 115 mg, 95% CI 109 to 120 mg; comparison mean: 117 mg, 95% CI 111 to 121 mg). However, unlike the small differences in trial 1 for vegetable intake, in trial 2 there was almost no difference in consumption, with the teacher-led group consuming on average 93 g of vegetables (95% CI 86 to 99 g) and the comparison group consuming on average 98 g (95% CI 90 to 104 g). There was a small difference of 8 g in fruit consumption, with the comparison group consuming slightly more fruit than the teacher-led group (teacher-led mean: 204 g, 95% CI 190 to 216 g; comparison group mean: 196 g, 95% CI 183 to 208 g).
Nutrient or food | Teacher-led (n = 667) | Comparison group (n = 698) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | |
Nutrient | ||||||
Energy (kcal) | 2034 | 28.0 | 1979 to 2089 | 1970 | 26.4 | 1917 to 2021 |
Energy (KJ) | 8554 | 117.4 | 8323 to 8784 | 8281 | 110.3 | 8064 to 8497 |
Protein (g) | 74 | 1.2 | 71 to 76 | 72 | 1.2 | 69 to 73 |
Carbohydrate (g) | 267 | 3.3 | 260 to 273 | 256 | 3.0 | 250 to 262 |
Fibre (Englyst) (g) | 13 | 0.2 | 12 to 13 | 12 | 0.2 | 11 to 12 |
Fat (g) | 82 | 1.6 | 78 to 85 | 80 | 1.6 | 77 to 83 |
Total sugars (g) | 133 | 1.9 | 128 to 136 | 128 | 1.9 | 123 to 131 |
Iron (mg) | 11 | 0.2 | 10 to 11 | 11 | 0.2 | 10 to 11 |
Calcium (mg) | 873 | 14.8 | 843 to 902 | 816 | 14.1 | 788 to 843 |
Potassium (mg) | 2723 | 39.2 | 2646 to 2800 | 2645 | 36.0 | 2574 to 2715 |
Sodium (mg) | 2710 | 51.7 | 2608 to 2811 | 2601 | 51.2 | 2500 to 2701 |
Folate (µg) | 232 | 3.9 | 224 to 239 | 220 | 3.5 | 212 to 226 |
Carotene (µg) | 1979 | 63.7 | 1853 to 2103 | 2137 | 66.5 | 2006 to 2267 |
Vitamin A (retinol equivalent) (µg) | 408 | 17.3 | 373 to 441 | 399 | 18.6 | 362 to 435 |
Vitamin C (mg) | 115 | 2.7 | 109 to 120 | 117 | 2.5 | 111 to 121 |
Food | ||||||
Total vegetables (excluding pulses, beans, lentils, dahl or seeds) (g) | 93 | 3.3 | 86 to 99 | 98 | 3.4 | 90 to 104 |
Pulses, beans, seeds (g) | 18 | 1.7 | 14 to 21 | 10 | 1.1 | 7 to 11 |
Total fruit (g) | 204 | 6.6 | 190 to 216 | 196 | 6.5 | 183 to 208 |
Fruit (non-dried) (g) | 203 | 6.6 | 190 to 216 | 195 | 6.5 | 181 to 207 |
Dried fruit (g) | 1 | 0.3 | 0.6 to 1 | 1 | 0.3 | 0.7 to 1 |
Total fruit and vegetables (excluding pulses and beans) (g) | 297 | 7.8 | 281 to 312 | 294 | 7.7 | 278 to 308 |
Sweets, toffees, mints (g) | 4 | 0.4 | 3 to 4 | 4 | 0.4 | 3 to 5 |
Chocolate bars (g) | 6 | 0.6 | 5 to 8 | 6 | 0.6 | 5 to 7 |
Crisps, savoury snacks (g) | 12 | 0.6 | 10 to 12 | 12 | 0.6 | 10 to 13 |
Milk or milky drinks (ml) | 111 | 5.6 | 99 to 121 | 95 | 5.5 | 84 to 105 |
Fizzy pop, squash, fruit drinks (ml) | 192 | 8.8 | 174 to 208 | 207 | 8.9 | 189 to 224 |
Fruit juice (pure) (ml) | 134 | 6.5 | 122 to 146 | 130 | 5.7 | 118 to 141 |
The nutrient and food intake for all children who completed trial 1, compared with children who did not complete the whole trial, is shown in Table 23. Overall, these results reveal that there was very little difference for key nutrients and foods between children who completed trial 1 baseline and follow-up and children who did not complete follow-up. The most noticeable difference was for mean energy intake, with children who completed the trial consuming, on average, 196 kcal less than children who did not complete the trial (completers: 1936 kcal, 95% CI 1879 to 1994 kcal; non-completers: 2090 kcal, 95% CI 2010 to 2169 kcal). However, there was very little difference in mean vitamin C intake (completers: 102 mg, 95% CI 97 to 107 mg; non-completers: 103 mg, 95% CI 97 to 110 mg) and mean vegetable consumption (completers: 91 g, 95% CI 85 to 97 g; non-completers: 93 g, 95% CI 85 to 100 g). There was a small difference of 11 g in mean fruit intake, with the children who completed the trial consuming, on average, more fruit than children who did not complete the trial (completers: 200 g, 95% CI 187 to 213 g; non-completers: 189 g, 95% CI 172 to 206 g).
Nutrient or food | Participants who completed both baseline and follow-up collection (n = 641) | Participants who did not complete follow-up (baseline only) (n = 388) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | |
Nutrient | ||||||
Energy (kcal) | 1936 | 29.0 | 1879 to 1994 | 2090 | 40.3 | 2010 to 2169 |
Energy (KJ) | 8104 | 121.8 | 7908 to 8386 | 8787 | 168.9 | 8455 to 9119 |
Protein (g) | 71 | 1.2 | 68 to 73 | 76 | 1.8 | 72 to 79 |
Carbohydrate (g) | 256 | 3.5 | 249 to 263 | 269 | 4.6 | 260 to 278 |
Fibre (Englyst) (g) | 12 | 0.2 | 12 to 13 | 13 | 0.3 | 12 to 14 |
Fat (g) | 76 | 1.5 | 73 to 79 | 86 | 2.3 | 81 to 90 |
Total sugars (g) | 128 | 2.1 | 123 to 132 | 128 | 2.7 | 123 to 134 |
Iron (mg) | 10 | 0.1 | 10 to 11 | 11 | 0.2 | 10 to 11 |
Calcium (mg) | 827 | 15.0 | 797 to 856 | 880 | 19.8 | 841 to 919 |
Potassium (mg) | 2673 | 41.4 | 2591 to 2754 | 2799 | 55.9 | 2689 to 2909 |
Sodium (mg) | 2503 | 49.5 | 2406 to 2600 | 2767 | 76.1 | 2617 to 2916 |
Folate (µg) | 216 | 3.7 | 207 to 224 | 229 | 5.2 | 219 to 240 |
Carotene (µg) | 2078 | 69.9 | 1941 to 2215 | 2004 | 87.5 | 1831 to 2176 |
Vitamin A (retinol equivalent) (µg) | 386 | 16.5 | 354 to 419 | 424 | 22.0 | 381 to 467 |
Vitamin C (mg) | 102 | 2.5 | 97 to 107 | 103 | 3.3 | 97 to 110 |
Food | ||||||
Total vegetables (excluding pulses, beans, lentils, dahl or seeds) (g) | 91 | 3.0 | 85 to 97 | 93 | 4.0 | 85 to 100 |
Pulses, beans, seeds (g) | 18 | 1.6 | 15 to 21 | 21 | 2.5 | 15 to 25 |
Total fruit (g) | 200 | 6.6 | 187 to 213 | 189 | 8.7 | 172 to 206 |
Fruit (non-dried) (g) | 198 | 6.5 | 185 to 210 | 191 | 8.9 | 173 to 208 |
Dried fruit (g) | 3 | 0.4 | 2 to 3 | 1 | 0.2 | 0.1 to 1.1 |
Total fruit and vegetables (excluding pulses and beans) (g) | 303 | 7.6 | 287 to 317 | 282 | 10.2 | 261 to 302 |
Sweets, toffees, mints (g) | 4 | 0.4 | 3 to 5 | 4 | 0.5 | 3 to 5 |
Chocolate bars (g) | 8 | 0.6 | 6 to 9 | 9 | 0.9 | 7 to 10 |
Crisps, savoury snacks (g) | 11 | 0.6 | 9 to 12 | 11 | 0.8 | 9 to 12 |
Milk or milky drinks (ml) | 117 | 5.7 | 105 to 128 | 110 | 7.1 | 95 to 123 |
Fizzy pop, squash, fruit drinks (ml) | 157 | 8.0 | 141 to 172 | 172 | 102.3 | 152 to 192 |
Fruit juice (pure) (ml) | 111 | 5.5 | 99 to 121 | 107 | 6.9 | 93 to 120 |
Additional descriptive analysis was conducted to explore the baseline nutrient and food intake for children who did not complete follow-up by intervention allocation. These results again revealed very little difference for children who did not complete trial 1 by intervention allocation. As expected, there was a slight difference in energy consumption, with the teacher-led group consuming more than the RHS-led group (RHS-led mean: 2046 kcal, 95% CI 1922 to 2169 kcal; teacher-led mean: 2119 kcal, 95% CI 2015 to 2223 kcal). Similar findings were found for the primary outcome measures of fruit and vegetable intake, with the teacher-led group consuming slightly more (for vegetables, RHS-led mean: 85 g, 95% CI 71 to 97 g; teacher-led mean: 98 g, 95% CI 88 to 107 g, and for fruit, RHS-led mean: 167 g, 95% CI 140 to 192 g; teacher-led mean: 204 g, 95% CI 181 to 226 g).
The nutrient and food intake for all children who did not complete follow-up in trial 2 by intervention allocation revealed very little difference for children who did not complete the trial. There was, on average, only 10 kcal difference between the teacher-led and comparison groups (teacher-led mean: 2020 kcal, 95% CI 1912 to 2126 kcal; comparison group mean: 2030 kcal, 95% CI 1943 to 2116 kcal). Similar results were found for the primary outcome measures of fruit and vegetable intake, with the teacher-led group consuming slightly more (for vegetables, teacher-led mean: 95 g, 95% CI 85 to 104 g; comparison group mean: 87 g, 95% CI 74 to 99 g, and for fruit, teacher-led mean: 199 g, 95% CI 177 to 219 g; comparison mean: 195 g, 95% CI 170 to 218 g).
Mean nutrient and food intake at baseline for all children who completed trial 2 compared with children who did not complete the trial (Table 24) found similar results to trial 1. Overall, these results reveal that there was very little difference for key nutrients and foods between children who completed trial 2 baseline and follow-up and children who did not complete follow-up. Similar to trial 1, the most noticeable difference was for mean energy intake, with children who completed the trial consuming, on average, 135 kcal less than children who did not complete the trial (completers: 1891 kcal, 95% CI 1839 to 1942 kcal; non-completers: 2026 kcal, 95% CI 1959 to 2092 kcal). This difference, however, was smaller than the difference in kcal intake seen in trial 1. Again, for trial 2 there was very little difference in mean vitamin C intake, in this case only 1 mg (completers: 112 mg, 95% CI 107 to 116 mg; non-completers: 111 mg, 95% CI 105 to 117 mg); and there was no difference in mean vegetable consumption (completers: 92 g, 95% CI 86 to 98 g; non-completers: 92 g, 95% CI 84 to 99 g). There was, however, a small difference of 9 g in mean fruit intake, with the children who completed the trial consuming, on average, more fruit than children who did not complete the trial (completers: 190 g, 95% CI 179 to 201 g; non-completers: 199 g, 95% CI 182 to 214 g).
Nutrient or food | Participants who completed both baseline and follow-up collection (n = 916) | Participants who did not complete follow-up (baseline only) (n = 443) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | |
Nutrient | ||||||
Energy (kcal) | 1891 | 26.1 | 1839 to 1942 | 2026 | 34.1 | 1959 to 2092 |
Energy (KJ) | 7952 | 109.5 | 7736 to 8166 | 8517 | 142.5 | 8237 to 8797 |
Protein (g) | 68 | 1.1 | 66 to 70 | 74 | 1.5 | 71 to 77 |
Carbohydrate (g) | 248 | 3.2 | 215 to 254 | 262 | 3.9 | 254 to 269 |
Fibre (Englyst) (g) | 12 | 0.2 | 11 to 12 | 12 | 0.3 | 11 to 13 |
Fat (g) | 76 | 1.4 | 73 to 79 | 83 | 2.0 | 78 to 86 |
Total sugars (g) | 124 | 1.8 | 120 to 127 | 130 | 2.5 | 125 to 134 |
Iron (mg) | 10 | 0.2 | 10 to 10 | 11 | 0.2 | 10 to 11 |
Calcium (mg) | 804 | 13.3 | 778 to 830 | 839 | 17.9 | 803 to 874 |
Potassium (mg) | 2531 | 35.2 | 2461 to 2599 | 2726 | 49.1 | 2629 to 2822 |
Sodium (mg) | 2535 | 45.1 | 2443 to 2620 | 2634 | 66.9 | 2502 to 2765 |
Folate (µg) | 216 | 3.4 | 209 to 223 | 222 | 4.7 | 212 to 231 |
Carotene (µg) | 1953 | 55.0 | 1844 to 2060 | 2070 | 81.8 | 1908 to 2230 |
Vitamin A (retinol equivalent) (µg) | 368 | 13.6 | 341 to 394 | 438 | 26.1 | 386 to 488 |
Vitamin C (mg) | 112 | 2.3 | 107 to 116 | 111 | 3.1 | 105 to 117 |
Food | ||||||
Total vegetables (excluding pulses, beans, lentils, dahl or seeds) (g) | 92 | 2.9 | 86 to 98 | 92 | 3.8 | 84 to 99 |
Pulses, beans, seeds (g) | 13 | 1.0 | 10 to 14 | 15 | 2.2 | 10 to 19 |
Total fruit (g) | 190 | 5.6 | 179 to 201 | 199 | 8.0 | 182 to 214 |
Fruit (non-dried) (g) | 190 | 5.6 | 178 to 200 | 197 | 8.0 | 181 to 212 |
Dried fruit (g) | 1 | 0.2 | 0.6 to 1 | 2 | 0.4 | 0.7 to 2 |
Total fruit and vegetables (excluding pulses and beans) (g) | 297 | 6.7 | 284 to 310 | 290 | 6.7 | 271 to 309 |
Sweets, toffees, mints (g) | 4 | 0.3 | 3 to 4 | 4 | 0.5 | 2 to 4 |
Chocolate bars (g) | 6 | 0.5 | 4 to 6 | 8 | 0.8 | 6 to 9 |
Crisps, savoury snacks (g) | 11 | 0.5 | 9 to 11 | 13 | 0.8 | 11 to 14 |
Milk or milky drinks (ml) | 94 | 4.5 | 84 to 102 | 110 | 7.4 | 95 to 124 |
Fizzy pop, squash, fruit drinks (ml) | 186 | 7.2 | 171 to 199 | 208 | 11.3 | 185 to 230 |
Fruit juice (pure) (ml) | 133 | 5.1 | 122 to 142 | 116 | 6.7 | 103 to 129 |
The baseline nutrient and food intake for all children who did complete baseline and follow-up in trial 1 showed small differences (Table 25), with a mean energy intake for the RHS-led group of 2034 kcal (95% CI 1956 to 2111 kcal) compared with the teacher-led mean intake of 1993 kcal (95% CI 1925 to 2059 kcal). There was only 2 g difference in mean carbohydrates intake (RHS-led mean: 265 g, 95% CI 256 to 273 g; teacher-led mean: 267 g, 95% CI 259 to 275 g) and 3 mg difference in vitamin C intake (RHS-led mean: 108 mg, 95% CI 100 to 115 mg; teacher-led mean: 105 mg, 95% CI 98 to 112 mg). There was a small difference in fruit and vegetable intake, with the teacher-led group consuming on average more vegetables (RHS-led mean: 87 g, 95% CI 78 to 95 g; teacher-led mean: 102 g, 95% CI 93 to 110 g) and more total fruit (RHS-led mean: 201 g, 95% CI 183 to 219 g; teacher-led mean: 214 g, 95% CI 195 to 232 g). This difference in intake was also noted in the 5 A DAY variable (RHS-led mean: 342 g, 95% CI 319 to 364 g; teacher-led mean: 374 g, 95% CI 347 to 382 g). The baseline nutrient and food intakes overall, however, are very similar in terms of levels of nutrients; this would suggest there was no evidence of imbalance between the groups.
Nutrient or food | RHS-led (n = 312) | Teacher-led (n = 329) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | |
Nutrient | ||||||
Energy (kcal) | 2034 | 39.4 | 1956 to 2111 | 1993 | 34.1 | 1925 to 2059 |
Energy (KJ) | 8552 | 164.9 | 8227 to 8876 | 8375 | 143.0 | 8103 to 8666 |
Protein (g) | 75 | 1.8 | 71 to 78 | 73 | 1.5 | 69 to 75 |
Carbohydrate (g) | 265 | 4.4 | 256 to 273 | 267 | 4.3 | 259 to 275 |
Fibre (Englyst) (g) | 13 | 0.3 | 12 to 13 | 13 | 0.3 | 12 to 13 |
Fat (g) | 82 | 2.3 | 77 to 86 | 78 | 1.7 | 74 to 81 |
Total sugars (g) | 132 | 2.9 | 126 to 137 | 134 | 2.6 | 128 to 138 |
Iron (mg) | 11 | 0.2 | 10 to 11 | 11 | 0.2 | 10 to 11 |
Calcium (mg) | 861 | 21.6 | 818 to 903 | 858 | 18.7 | 821 to 895 |
Potassium (mg) | 2771 | 54.7 | 2663 to 2878 | 2784 | 51.3 | 2683 to 2884 |
Sodium (mg) | 2632 | 76.3 | 2481 to 2782 | 2572 | 57.6 | 2458 to 2685 |
Folate (µg) | 227 | 5.3 | 216 to 237 | 224 | 4.5 | 214 to 232 |
Carotene (µg) | 1956 | 98.8 | 1765 to 2146 | 2352 | 101.7 | 2152 to 2552 |
Vitamin A (retinol equivalent) (µg) | 400 | 25.1 | 350 to 449 | 403 | 22.7 | 358 to 448 |
Vitamin C (mg) | 108 | 3.7 | 100 to 115 | 105 | 3.5 | 98 to 112 |
Food | ||||||
Total vegetables (excluding pulses, beans, lentils, dahl or seeds) (g) | 87 | 4.4 | 78 to 95 | 102 | 4.3 | 93 to 110 |
Pulses, beans, seeds (g) | 16 | 2.2 | 12 to 20 | 21 | 2.4 | 16 to 25 |
Total fruit (g) | 201 | 9.3 | 183 to 219 | 214 | 9.5 | 195 to 232 |
Fruit (non-dried) (g) | 201 | 9.1 | 182 to 218 | 211 | 9.5 | 191 to 229 |
Dried fruit (g) | 3 | 0.6 | 1 to 3 | 3 | 0.7 | 2 to 4 |
Total fruit and vegetables (excluding pulses and beans) (g) | 269 | 10.7 | 248 to 290 | 300 | 10.5 | 278 to 320 |
Sweets, toffees, mints (g) | 5 | 0.7 | 3 to 6 | 4 | 0.5 | 2 to 4 |
Chocolate bars (g) | 9 | 1.0 | 6 to 10 | 7 | 0.9 | 5 to 9 |
Crisps, savoury snacks (g) | 12 | 1.0 | 10 to 14 | 10 | 0.8 | 8 to 11 |
Milk or milky drinks (ml) | 138 | 8.9 | 120 to 153 | 106 | 7.6 | 91 to 120 |
Fizzy pop, squash, fruit drinks (ml) | 163 | 11.4 | 141 to 185 | 163 | 11.8 | 139 to 185 |
Fruit juice (pure) (ml) | 119 | 8.5 | 102 to 135 | 112 | 7.6 | 95 to 126 |
Table 26 shows the baseline nutrient and food intake for all children who did complete baseline and follow-up for trial 2. At baseline, the values for key foods, nutrients and energy were all closely matched across the two groups. Similar to trial 1, there was a small difference in mean energy intake between the two groups, with the teacher-led group consuming, on average, 2039 kcal (95% CI 1974 to 2103 kcal) and the comparison group consuming, on average, 1932 kcal (95% CI 1867 to 1996 kcal). There was only 13 g difference in mean carbohydrates intake (teacher-led mean: 267 g, 95% CI 259 to 275 g; comparison group mean: 254 g, 95% CI 246 to 275 g) and no difference in vitamin C intake (teacher-led mean: 118 mg, 95% CI 111 to 124 mg; comparison mean: 118 mg, 95% CI 111 to 124 mg). Again, there were similar results for the groups in trial 2 for fruit and vegetable consumption, with only small differences between the groups. The teacher-led group consumed, on average, less vegetables (teacher-led mean: 95 g, 95% CI 87 to 102 g; comparison group mean: 100 g, 95% CI 90 to 108 g) and more fruit (teacher-led mean: 206 g, 95% CI 190 to 221 g; comparison group mean: 193 g, 95% CI 177 to 209 g).
Nutrient or food | Teacher-led (n = 488) | Comparison group (n = 428) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | |
Nutrient | ||||||
Energy (kcal) | 2039 | 32.7 | 1974 to 2103 | 1932 | 32.8 | 1867 to 1996 |
Energy (KJ) | 8576 | 137.3 | 8306 to 8845 | 8125 | 137.3 | 7854 to 8394 |
Protein (g) | 75 | 1.4 | 71 to 77 | 69 | 1.4 | 66 to 72 |
Carbohydrate (g) | 267 | 4.0 | 259 to 275 | 254 | 3.6 | 246 to 261 |
Fibre (Englyst) (g) | 13 | 0.3 | 12 to 13 | 12 | 0.2 | 11 to 12 |
Fat (g) | 82 | 18.0 | 78 to 85 | 78 | 2.0 | 74 to 82 |
Total sugars (g) | 133 | 2.3 | 128 to 137 | 127 | 2.4 | 122 to 132 |
Iron (mg) | 11 | 0.2 | 10 to 11 | 11 | 0.2 | 10 to 11 |
Calcium (mg) | 877 | 17.6 | 842 to 911 | 810 | 17.5 | 775 to 844 |
Potassium (mg) | 2730 | 45.0 | 2642 to 2818 | 2585 | 43.4 | 2499 to 2670 |
Sodium (mg) | 2742 | 58.4 | 2627 to 2990 | 2575 | 64.2 | 2448 to 2700 |
Folate (µg) | 235 | 4.5 | 225 to 243 | 220 | 4.3 | 211 to 228 |
Carotene (µg) | 2024 | 74.9 | 1876 to 2170 | 2089 | 83.9 | 1924 to 2254 |
Vitamin A (retinol equivalent) (µg) | 398 | 19 | 361 to 434 | 374 | 21.1 | 332 to 415 |
Vitamin C (mg) | 118 | 3.2 | 111 to 124 | 118 | 3.2 | 111 to 124 |
Food | ||||||
Total vegetables (excluding pulses, beans, lentils, dahl or seeds) (g) | 95 | 3.8 | 87 to 102 | 100 | 4.7 | 90 to 108 |
Pulses, beans, seeds (g) | 16 | 1.6 | 12 to 19 | 10 | 1.4 | 7 to 13 |
Total fruit (g) | 206 | 7.9 | 190 to 221 | 193 | 8.2 | 177 to 209 |
Fruit (non-dried) (g) | 206 | 7.9 | 190 to 221 | 192 | 8.2 | 176 to 208 |
Dried fruit (g) | 1 | 0.3 | 0.5 to 1 | 1 | 0.3 | 0.4 to 1 |
Total fruit and vegetables (excluding pulses and beans) (g) | 299 | 8.9 | 282 to 317 | 296 | 9.6 | 277 to 314 |
Sweets, toffees, mints (g) | 4 | 0.5 | 3 to 5 | 4 | 0.6 | 3 to 5 |
Chocolate bars (g) | 6 | 0.7 | 4 to 7 | 6 | 0.7 | 4 to 7 |
Crisps, savoury snacks (g) | 12 | 0.7 | 10 to 13 | 11 | 0.7 | 9 to 12 |
Milk or milky drinks (ml) | 101 | 6.2 | 89 to 113 | 97 | 7.0 | 82 to 110 |
Fizzy pop, squash, fruit drinks (ml) | 189 | 10.1 | 168 to 208 | 203 | 11.1 | 181 to 224 |
Fruit juice (pure) (ml) | 141 | 7.4 | 126 to 155 | 138 | 7.5 | 123 to 152 |
Change in fruit and vegetable intake: trial 1
Table 27 displays the changes in fruit intake, vegetable intake and combined fruit and vegetable intake (follow-up minus baseline) and the intervention mean difference for trial 1, both unadjusted and adjusted for IMDS, age, gender and ethnicity. For both groups, there was a small decrease in fruit intake after adjusting for possible confounders (RHS-led mean: 8 g, 95% CI: –69 to 52 g; teacher-led mean: 20 g, 95% CI –36 to 77 g). For vegetable consumption there were no significant differences found between the unadjusted and adjusted models (intervention effect: –13 g, 95% CI –39 to 11 g). The teacher-led group did have, on average, a higher mean change in vegetable consumption, of 29 g (95% CI –6 to 66 g) compared with 16 g (95% CI –11 to 38 g) in the RHS-led group.
Food | RHS-led (n = 312) | Teacher-led (n = 328) | Intervention effect | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean difference | SE | 95% CI | p-value | |
Unadjusted | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | –33 | 11.8 | –56 to –10 | –6 | 11.5 | –28 to 16 | 27 | 16.4 | –5 to 59 | 0.1 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 2 | 9.0 | –15 to 20 | 16 | 8.6 | –1 to 32 | –13 | 12.4 | –38 to 11 | 0.3 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | –32 | 14.5 | –60 to –3 | 8 | 14.0 | –19 to 36 | 40 | 20.2 | –1 to 80 | 0.05 |
Adjusteda | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | –8 | 30.8 | –69 to 52 | –20 | 29.0 | –36 to 77 | –28 | 16.4 | –60 to 3 | 0.08 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 16 | 19.6 | –11 to 38 | 29 | 18.2 | –6 to 66 | –13 | 12.8 | –39 to 11 | 0.2 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 1 | 39.4 | –75 to 78 | 44 | 36.7 | –27 to 116 | –43 | 22.8 | –88 to 1 | 0.06 |
For combined fruit and vegetable intake there was a borderline significant difference in the unadjusted model (intervention effect: 40 g, 95% CI –1 to 80 g; p = 0.05), with the teacher-led group having a higher mean change of 8 g (95% CI –19 to 36 g) and the RHS-led group a mean change of –32 g (95% CI –60 to –3 g). However, after adjusting for possible confounders this difference was not significant (intervention effect: –42 g, 95% CI –88 to 1 g; p = 0.06).
The plot of the school residuals with their 95% confidence limits are presented in ascending order in Figure 16. All of the schools do pass through zero, indicating that the schools do not differ significantly from the average line at the 5% level. From the adjusted model, results state that 1.2% of the variance in change in mean fruit and vegetable intake can be attributed to the difference between schools.
Change in fruit and vegetable intake: trial 2
Table 28 displays the changes in fruit intake, vegetable intake and combined fruit and vegetable intake at baseline and follow-up and the intervention mean difference for trial 2, both unadjusted and adjusted for IMDS, age, gender and ethnicity. For mean change in fruit intake, the teacher-led group (mean change 44 g, 95% CI –28 to 118 g) increased their consumption, on average, by 22 g more than the comparison group (mean change 22 g, 95% CI –50 to 94 g). However, these differences in mean change for fruit intake were not significant in the unadjusted or adjusted models. For vegetable intake, the comparison group consumed, on average, more vegetables (mean change 17 g, 95% CI –30 to 21 g) compared with the teacher-led group (mean change 10 g, 95% CI –36 to 52 g). However, this difference was not significant.
Food | Teacher-led (n = 488) | Comparison group (n = 428) | Intervention effect | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean difference | SE | 95% CI | p-value | |
Unadjusted | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | 13 | 17.6 | –20.9 to 48.2 | –12 | 17.1 | –45.9 to 21.0 | 26 | 24.5 | –21.0 to 74.0 | 0.3 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 16 | 10.2 | –3.8 to 36.1 | 22 | 9.9 | 1.9 to 40.7 | –5 | 14.2 | –22.7 to 33.0 | 0.7 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 29.8 | 23.0 | –15.3 to 74.9 | 9.0 | 22.4 | –34.8 to 52.8 | 20 | 32.1 | –83.7 to 42.1 | 0.5 |
Adjusteda | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | 44 | 37.5 | –28 to 118 | 22 | 36.9 | –50 to 94 | –22 | 24.3 | –70 to 24 | 0.3 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 10 | 21.3 | –36 to 52 | 17 | 20.9 | –30 to 21 | –7 | 14.2 | –35 to 20 | 0.6 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 56 | 47.1 | –36 to 148 | 40 | 46.4 | –50 to 131 | 15 | 32.0 | –36 to 148 | 0.6 |
As a result of having a higher intake of fruit, the teacher-led group consumed on average 15 g (95% CI –36 to 148 g) more fruit and vegetables than the comparison group. This difference was not significant in either the adjusted or the unadjusted model.
The plots of the residuals with their 95% confidence limits are presented in ascending order in Figure 17. It is evident that the majority of the schools do pass through zero, indicating that they do not differ significantly from the average line at the 5% level. The adjusted model of mean change in combined fruit and vegetable results shows that 7.3% of the variance in mean change in fruit and vegetable intake can be attributed to the difference between schools.
Differences in nutrients and key foods
For both trials, the differences in key nutrients and foods were explored to see if there was an effect of either intervention on their mean intakes. Results are presented for trials 1 (Table 29) and 2 (Table 30), both unadjusted and adjusted for age, gender, ethnicity and IMDS. Overall, there was very little difference in either trial for these key nutrients and foods. The mean differences were small for nearly all nutrients and foods, except for energy and carotene intake. Although there were differences in mean intakes for these two, they were not significant. The only significant difference was found in trial 1 for vitamin C intake in the adjusted model. Once the adjustments were made there was a 12.7-mg-per-day difference between the RHS-led and teacher-led groups, with the teacher-led group having a significantly higher intake of vitamin C.
Nutrient intake | RHS-led (n = 312) | Teacher-led (n = 329) | Intervention effect | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean difference | SE | 95% CI | p-value | |
Unadjusted | ||||||||||
Total energy (KJ/day) | 7266 | 525 | 6238 to 8294 | 7389 | 506 | 6396 to 8381 | –123 | 435 | –730 to 976 | 0.8 |
Total energy (kcal/day) | 1730 | 124.8 | 1485 to 1974 | 1758 | 120 | 1522 to 1994 | –28 | 104 | –175 to 232 | 0.8 |
Fat (g/day) | 75.3 | 5.4 | 64.8 to 85.8 | 73.3 | 5.1 | 63.4 to 83.2 | 2.0 | 5.5 | –12.8 to 8.7 | 0.7 |
Salt (mg/day) | 2426 | 179.2 | 2075 to 2777 | 2395 | 171 | 2060 to 2729 | 31 | 189 | –401 to 339 | 0.9 |
Sugars (g/day) including non-milk extrinsic sugars | 87.4 | 6.7 | 74.2 to 100.5 | 96.3 | 6.7 | 83.2 to 109.5 | 9 | 5.2 | –1.2 to 19.2 | 0.08 |
Carotene (µg/day) | 1788 | 189 | 1417 to 2159 | 1968 | 188 | 1599 to 2337 | 144 | 232 | –340 to 629 | 0.4 |
Vitamin C (mg/day) | 74.8 | 6.1 | 62.9 to 86.7 | 87.8 | 5.9 | 76.3 to 99.4 | 13.0 | 5.7 | 1.8 to 24.2 | 0.2 |
Iron (µg/day) | 9.1 | 0.7 | 7.8 to 10.5 | 9.4 | 0.7 | 8.1 to 10.7 | –0.3 | 0.3 | –0.9 to 1.4 | 0.8 |
Fibre (g/day) | 11.7 | 0.9 | 1.0 to 13.4 | 12.8 | 0.9 | 11.1 to 14.5 | –1.2 | 0.8 | –0.5 to 2.8 | 0.2 |
Carbohydrates (g/day) | 213.7 | 15.4 | 183.6 to 243.9 | 219.3 | 15.3 | 189.3 to 249.2 | –5.5 | 10.8 | –15.5 to 26.6 | 0.6 |
Folate (µg/day) | 180.0 | 12.5 | 155.4 to 204.5 | 189.8 | 12.1 | 166.0 to 213.6 | –9.9 | 10.9 | –11.3 to 31.1 | 0.4 |
Protein (g/day) | 64.4 | 4.7 | 55.1 to 73.6 | 69.6 | 4.5 | 60.8 to 78.3 | 5.2 | 4.7 | –4.0 to 14.3 | 0.3 |
Adjusteda | ||||||||||
Total energy (KJ/day) | 6387 | 749 | 4920 to 7855 | 6587 | 708 | 5199 to 7974 | –199 | 430 | –1043 to 644 | 0.6 |
Total energy (kcal/day) | 1520 | 178 | 1171 to 1870 | 1567 | 168 | 1237 to 1897 | –46 | 103 | –247 to 154 | 0.6 |
Fat (g/day) | 65 | 8.2 | 49 to 81 | 64 | 7.7 | 49 to 79 | 1 | 5.2 | –9 to 11 | 0.8 |
Salt (g/day) | 2272 | 286 | 1711 to 2833 | 2257 | 267.7 | 1732 to 2781 | 16 | 190.4 | –357 to 388 | 0.9 |
Sugars (g/day) including non-milk extrinsic sugars | 90 | 10.5 | 70 to 111 | 99 | 10.0 | 80 to 118 | –8 | 5.1 | –18 to 2 | 0.1 |
Carotene (µg/day) | 1694 | 213 | 1249 to 2139 | 1834 | 262 | 1288 to 2380 | –140 | 199 | –2752 to 556 | 0.5 |
Vitamin C (mg/day) | 113 | 31.7 | 51 to 175 | 125 | 31 | 64 to 187 | 13 | 5.5 | 2.0 to 23.5 | 0.02 |
Iron (µg/day) | 8 | 1.0 | 6 to 10 | 8 | 0.9 | 6 to 10 | –0.4 | 0.6 | –1 to 0.9 | 0.5 |
Fibre (g/day) | 10 | 1.3 | 7 to 13 | 11 | 1.3 | 9 to 14 | –1 | 0.8 | –3 to 1 | 0.1 |
Carbohydrates (g/day) | 186 | 21.5 | 144 to 228 | 193 | 20.6 | 153 to 234 | –7 | 10.9 | –28 to 14 | 0.5 |
Folate (µg/day) | 169 | 19.7 | 131 to 208 | 180 | 18.6 | 144 to 217 | –11 | 10.9 | –32 to 10 | 0.3 |
Protein (g/day) | 58 | 7.1 | 44 to 72 | 64 | 6.7 | 51 to 77 | –6 | 4.8 | –15 to 3 | 0.2 |
Nutrient intake | Teacher-led (n = 488) | Comparison group (n = 428) | Intervention effect | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean difference | SE | 95% CI | p-value | |
Unadjusted | ||||||||||
Total energy (KJ/day) | 7848 | 454 | 6958 to 8739 | 7807 | 464 | 6898 to 8715 | 42 | 404 | –833 to 749 | 0.9 |
Total energy (kcal/day) | 1868 | 108 | 1657 to 2080 | 1860 | 110 | 1642 to 2075 | 10 | 96 | –198 to 179 | 0.9 |
Fat (g/day) | 81.5 | 4.5 | 72.7 to 90.3 | 82.2 | 4.5 | 73 to 91 | –1 | 4.9 | –8.9 to 10.3 | 0.9 |
Salt (mg/day) | 2708 | 145 | 2424 to 2991 | 2746 | 146 | 2459 to 3032 | –38 | 158 | –262 to 338 | 0.8 |
Sugars (g/day) including non-milk extrinsic sugars | 88 | 6.7 | 75.0 to 101.2 | 87 | 6.8 | 74 to 100 | 1.2 | 5.8 | –12.7 to 10.2 | 0.8 |
Carotene (µg/day) | 2032 | 223 | 1567 to 2480 | 2358 | 246 | 1853 to 2862 | –335 | 301 | –952 to 282 | 0.2 |
Vitamin C (mg/day) | 92.6 | 6.1 | 80.6 to 104.5 | 90.9 | 5.9 | 79 to 103 | –1.7 | 6.3 | –14.0 to 10.6 | 0.8 |
Iron (µg/day) | 10.7 | 0.6 | 9.5 to 11.8 | 10.5 | 0.6 | 9 to 11 | 0.1 | 0.6 | –1.3 to 10 | 0.8 |
Fibre (g/day) | 11.9 | 0.8 | 10.5 to 13.4 | 11.6 | 0.8 | 10 to 13 | 0.3 | 0.8 | –1.8 to 1.2 | 0.7 |
Carbohydrates (g/day) | 219.0 | 14.0 | 191.6 to 246.4 | 216.4 | 14.3 | 188 to 244 | 3 | 11.4 | –24.9 to 19.7 | 0.8 |
Folate (µg/day) | 201.5 | 10.7 | 180.4 to 222.5 | 198.1 | 10.9 | 177 to 219 | 3.4 | 10.2 | –23.3 to 16.6 | 0.7 |
Protein (g/day) | 70.7 | 3.9 | 63.2 to 78.3 | 68 | 3.9 | 61 to 76 | 2 | 3.9 | –9.5 to 5.7 | 0.6 |
Adjusteda | ||||||||||
Total energy (KJ/day) | 7761 | 720 | 6349 to 9174 | 7719 | 717 | 6313 to 9125 | 42 | 404 | –751 to 835 | 0.9 |
Total energy (kcal/day) | 1845 | 172 | 1509 to 2182 | 1836 | 170 | 1501 to 2170 | 9 | 95.5 | –179 to 198 | 0.9 |
Fat (g/day) | 76 | 7.9 | 60 to 91 | 77 | 7.9 | 61 to 92 | –1 | 4.8 | –10 to 8 | 0.8 |
Salt (g/day) | 2621 | 259 | 2113 to 3129 | 2656 | 257 | 2152 to 3159 | –34 | 152 | –332 to 263 | 0.8 |
Sugars (g/day) including non-milk extrinsic sugars | 108 | 11.4 | 75 to 126 | 107 | 11.3 | 85 to 129 | 1 | 5.7 | –10 to 12 | 0.8 |
Carotene (µg/day) | 1841 | 299 | 1227 to 2456 | 2168 | 329 | 1493 to 2843 | –327 | 295 | –932 to 279 | 0.2 |
Vitamin C (mg/day) | 75 | 30.2 | 16 to 134 | 73 | 30 | 14 to 132 | 2 | 6 | –14 to 9 | 0.7 |
Iron (µg/day) | 10 | 0.9 | 8 to 12 | 10 | 0.9 | 8 to 12 | 0.1 | 0.6 | –1 to 1.2 | 0.8 |
Fibre (g/day) | 12 | 1.2 | 9 to 14 | 11 | 1.2 | 9 to 14 | 0.3 | 0.8 | –1 to 2 | 0.6 |
Carbohydrates (g/day) | 227 | 21.7 | 184 to 270 | 225 | 21.6 | 182 to 267 | 2 | 11.4 | –20 to 24 | 0.8 |
Folate (µg/day) | 192 | 18.9 | 155 to 229 | 188 | 18.8 | 151 to 225 | 4 | 10.2 | –15 to 24 | 0.6 |
Protein (g/day) | 70 | 6.5 | 58 to 83 | 68 | 6.4 | 56 to 81 | 2 | 3.9 | –6 to 9 | 0.6 |
Differences in food and drink intake
An additional analysis was conducted to determine if there were differences in non-essential food intake (sweets, toffees, mints, chocolate bars, crisps, savoury snacks) and commonly consumed drinks (milk; fizzy pop, squash, fruit drink and pure fruit juice). For both trials, no differences were found in intakes of non-essential foods or drinks, after adjusting for age, gender, ethnicity and IMDS. Overall, there was very little difference between the different intervention groups. In trial 1, the RHS-led group consumed, on average, 19 ml less milk (95% CI –49 to 11 ml) than the teacher-led group. However, this difference was not significant.
Sensitivity analysis
Baseline values brought forward
Sensitivity analysis was carried out using baseline data brought forward to explore the effect on the primary outcome. The results from this analysis are presented for trial 1 in Table 31 and for trial 2 in Table 32. The same methodology used to explore the intervention effect in the main analysis was applied to baseline values brought forward. There was very little difference in the results for baseline brought forward compared with the main trial analysis for trial 1. Instead of having a decrease in mean change in fruit intake, there is almost no change (2.0 g) for the RHS-led group and a change of 10.7 g for the teacher-led group. The mean difference in vegetable intake increases from 13 g to 35 g; however, after the adjustments are made this difference is not significant. The difference in combined change in fruit and vegetable intake was negligible between the main ITT model and baseline brought forward for trial 1.
Food | RHS-led (n = 465) | Teacher-led (n = 563) | Intervention effect | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean difference | SE | 95% CI | p-value | |
Unadjusted | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | 2 | 6.9 | –11.5 to 15.4 | 10.7 | 6.2 | –1.4 to 22.9 | –8.8 | 9.3 | –9.4 to 27.0 | 0.3 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 177 | 15.7 | 146.3 to 207.9 | 212.1 | 14.2 | 184.2 to 240.0 | –35.0 | 21.2 | –76.5 to 6.6 | 0.1 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | –14.1 | 8.8 | –31.3 to 3.1 | 5.2 | 7.9 | –10.4 to 20.7 | –19.2 | 11.8 | –42.4 to 4.0 | 0.1 |
Adjusted a | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | 17 | 17.2 | –16 to 51 | 27 | 16.7 | –5 to 60 | –10 | 9.2 | –28 to 7 | 0.2 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 141 | 29.3 | 84 to 199 | 180 | 28.1 | 125 to 506 | –38 | 22.0 | –81 to 4.8 | 0.08 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 172 | 71.3 | 33 to 312 | 195 | 71.4 | 55 to 335 | –22 | 12.0 | –46 to 1 | 0.06 |
Food | Teacher-led (n = 667) | Comparison group (n = 698) | Intervention effect | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean difference | SE | 95% CI | p-value | |
Unadjusted | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | 27.4 | 10.0 | 7.9 to 46.9 | 12.5 | 10.0 | –7.2 to 32.2 | –14.9 | 14.1 | –42.6 to 12.8 | 0.3 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 224.2 | 12.7 | 199.4 to 249.1 | 211.5 | 12.8 | 186.4 to 236.6 | 12.7 | 18.0 | –48.0 to 22.6 | 0.5 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 28.3 | 13.7 | 1.4 to 55.2 | 8.8 | 13.8 | –18.3 to 36.0 | –19.5 | 19.5 | –57.7 to 18.7 | 0.3 |
Adjusted a | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | 44 | 23.8 | –2 to 91 | 32 | 23.6 | –14 to 78 | 13 | 14.0 | –14 to 40 | 0.3 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 227 | 30.8 | 166 to 287 | 208 | 30.6 | 148 to 268 | 18 | 17.9 | –16 to 54 | 0.3 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 49 | 31.8 | –12 to 112 | 32 | 31.6 | –29 to 94 | 17 | 19.2 | –20 to 55 | 0.4 |
The plots of the residuals with their 95% confidence limits are presented in ascending order in Figure 18. There was even less divergence from zero for all the schools, indicating that the schools do not differ significantly from the average line at the 5% level. From the adjusted model of the mean change in combined fruit and vegetable intake, results state that 0.1% of the variance in change in mean fruit and vegetable intake can be attributed to the difference between schools.
For trial 2, displayed in Table 32, differences in the main analysis and the baseline brought forward are minor, with only a slight decrease in all three mean differences for fruit, vegetables and combined fruit and vegetable intake. Again, once adjusted for the covariates, these differences in mean intakes were not significant.
Figure 19 shows very little difference compared with the main analysis. The overall plot shows the majority of the schools do pass through zero, indicating that the schools do not differ significantly from the average line at the 5% level. From the adjusted model of the mean change in combined fruit and vegetable intake, results state that 3.8% of the variance in change in mean fruit and vegetable intake can be attributed to the difference between schools.
Differences between boys and girls by intervention allocation
Differences in fruit and vegetable intake between boys and girls by intervention allocation were explored. There is very little difference between boys and girls in the RHS-led group, with both showing a mean decrease in fruit consumption (girls: –34 g, 95% CI –68 to –1 g; boys: –31 g, 95% CI –64 to 2 g) and for vegetable consumption, almost no difference (girls: –1 g, 95% CI –17 to 30 g; boys: 5 g, 95% CI –14 to 25 g) (Table 33). For the combined mean change in fruit and vegetables, boys in the RHS-led group decreased their consumption less than girls (girls: –37 g, 95% CI –76 to 1.2 g; boys: –26 g, 95% CI –65 to 12 g). Results for the teacher-led schools revealed that the girls tended to consume more vegetables than boys (girls: mean change 28 g, 95% CI 6 to 49 g; boys: mean change 5 g, 95% CI –16 to 26 g). This difference was also reflected in combined fruit and vegetable intake, with girls on average having a mean change of 15 g (95% CI –63 to 55 g) in fruit and vegetable consumption, compared with 2 g change for boys (95% CI –36 to 63 g).
Food | Girls | Boys | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | |
RHS-led | ||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | –34 | 16.9 | –68 to –1 | –31 | 16.8 | –64 to 2 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | –1 | 10.1 | –17 to 30 | 5 | 10.1 | –14 to 25 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | –37 | 19.9 | –76 to 1.2 | –26 | 19.7 | –65 to 12 |
Teacher-led | ||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | –10 | 16.2 | –42 to 21 | –2 | 15.7 | –32 to 28 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 28 | 11.0 | 6 to 49 | 5 | 10.8 | –16 to 26 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 15 | 20.7 | –63 to 55 | 2 | 20.1 | –36 to 63 |
For trial 2, there was very little difference between the two groups in either the teacher-led intervention or the comparison group, with the teacher-led girls having a slightly higher mean change of 32 g (95% CI –27 to 91 g) in fruit and vegetable intake, compared with the boys’ mean change of 27 g (95% CI –32 to 87 g) (Table 34). In the comparison group there was a difference in fruit intake between boys and girls, with the girls having a decrease in mean intake of 9 g (95% CI –46 to 28 g) and the boys having a mean change of –17 g (95% CI –50 to 14 g). However, their vegetable and combined fruit and vegetable intakes were very similar.
Food | Girls | Boys | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | |
Teacher-led | ||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | 15 | 23.3 | –43 to 38 | 12 | 20.9 | –38 to 43 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 17 | 13.3 | –8 to 20 | 15 | 13.4 | –10 to 41 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 32 | 30.4 | –27 to 91 | 27 | 30.5 | –32 to 87 |
Comparison group | ||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | –9 | 15.9 | –46 to 28 | 17 | 16.6 | –50 to 14 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 24 | 9.3 | 6 to 42 | 18 | 9.0 | 0 to 36 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 9 | 19.4 | –29 to 46 | 6 | 20.1 | –33 to 46 |
Differences between boys and girls interaction effect
Additional analysis was conducted to explore whether or not there was an interaction between gender and the intervention. The results from this analysis are presented in Tables 35 and 36. After adjusting for age, ethnicity and IMDS, no interaction effect of gender was detected.
Food | RHS-led (n = 312) | Teacher-led (n = 329) | Interaction | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean difference | SE | 95% CI | p-value | |
Unadjusted | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | 4 | 23.5 | –42 to 49 | 9 | 22.9 | –36 to 53 | 5 | 32.8 | –59 to 69 | 0.8 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 6 | 12.1 | –1 to 58 | –22 | 11.7 | –45 to 0 | 29 | 16.8 | –3 to 62 | 0.08 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 11 | 27.5 | –43 to 64 | –13 | 26.8 | –65 to 39 | 24 | 38.4 | –76 to 2 | 0.06 |
Adjusteda | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | 11 | 23.3 | –34 to 56 | 15 | 22.6 | –29 to 59 | 3 | 32.3 | –66 to 16 | 0.9 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | 8 | 11.9 | –15 to 31 | –21 | 11.6 | –44 to 0 | 29 | 16.6 | –3 to 62 | 0.07 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 18 | 27.1 | –34 to 72 | –6 | 26.3 | –57 to 45 | 24 | 37.7 | –49 to 98 | 0.5 |
Food | Teacher-led (n = 488) | Comparison group (n = 428) | Interaction | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean | SE | 95% CI | Mean difference | SE | 95% CI | p-value | |
Unadjusted | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | –3 | 20.7 | –43 to 37 | 9 | 19.4 | –28 to 47 | –12 | 28.4 | –68 to 43 | 0.6 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | –2 | 11.3 | –24 to 20 | –7 | 10.5 | –27 to 14 | 4 | 15.5 | –26 to 34 | 0.7 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | –5 | 25.1 | –54 to 44 | 3 | 23.4 | –42 to 49 | –8 | 34.4 | –42 to 57 | 0.7 |
Adjusteda | ||||||||||
Change in fruit intake (g) | 5 | 20.4 | –35 to 46 | 16 | 19.0 | –20 to 53 | –11 | 27.8 | –66 to 42 | 0.6 |
Change in vegetable intake (g) | –3 | 11.4 | –24 to 40 | –5 | 10.6 | –25 to 16 | 2 | 15.5 | –28 to 32 | 0.9 |
Change in combined fruit and vegetable intake (g) | 2 | 24.9 | –46 to 50 | 12 | 23.1 | –33 to 57 | –10 | 33.8 | –76 to 56 | 0.7 |
Discussion
Fruit and vegetable consumption
The results from these trials revealed that there was very little difference in children’s mean change in fruit, vegetable or combined fruit and vegetable intake. For both trials, the teacher-led group had slightly higher mean intakes of vegetables and combined fruit and vegetables than the RHS-led or comparison group; however, there was no significant intervention effect after taking into consideration the adjustment for possible confounders. Only five other studies measured the relationship between children’s fruit and vegetable intake and a gardening intervention. 14,47,48,125,126 The results from these five studies were mixed, with two studies revealing a significant difference for fruit and vegetable intake,14,48 one125 finding that boys had a higher consumption of fruit and vegetables compared with girls, and one reporting a significant increase in vegetable consumption,126 while the fifth study found no differences in fruit or vegetable intake (measured separately only). 47
Of the four studies that did show an effect on fruit or vegetable intake, two used self-selection to determine which school received the intervention. 48,126 In one study, the teacher was able to choose whether they received the intervention or not,126 and the other was based on existing gardening activities within the schools. 48 One study47 stated that the head teacher chose which classes would receive the intervention, and the fourth study14 used convenient sampling for the three schools involved; two of the three schools were randomly assigned, whereas the third school was assigned based on garden availability. However, for both of the current trials, gardening area or existing activities was not a requirement and all schools were randomly assigned to their intervention group.
Nutrient consumption
For both trials, the differences in key nutrients and foods were explored to see if there was an effect of either intervention on their mean intakes. Overall, there was very little difference in either trial for these key nutrients and foods. The only significant difference was in trial 1 for vitamin C intake in the adjusted model. Once the adjustments were made there was a 13.0-mg-per-day difference between the RHS- and teacher-led groups, with the teacher-led group having a significantly higher intake of vitamin C. A previous study14 explored key nutrients, identifying significant increases in dietary fibre and vitamins A and C in the gardening intervention group compared with the control group.
Potential barriers to changing children’s fruit and vegetable consumption
Changing children’s fruit and vegetable consumption patterns is a challenging task. The main barriers to increasing children’s fruit and vegetable intake are availability, convenience, taste preferences, peer pressure, parental/school support and knowledge. 49 Successful implementation of an intervention is often determined by the time allocated and the teachers’ and parents’ perceptions of its importance. The main barrier for teachers in implementing school-based interventions is preparation time. 127 The teacher’s willingness to teach the intervention and own beliefs in the importance of the garden could explain the current findings that, although not significant, the teacher-led intervention tended to have a higher increase in fruit and vegetable consumption compared with the RHS-led intervention and the comparison group. Another important geographical component to acknowledge when evaluating the success of a gardening intervention is that all of the successful interventions were located in the USA, in areas where fruit and vegetables could be grown all year round. In the current research, the length of the growing season may have had an effect on the outcome. Further analysis exploring how the delivery and implementation of the intervention may have affected the primary outcome is described in subsequent chapters.
Limitations and strengths
One of the disadvantages of the design of this research was having two trials instead of one. Therefore, the difference between the RHS-led intervention and the comparison group could not be analysed. However, for both trials the study design was robust, using randomisation to determine which schools received the different interventions. This is the first clustered RCT to evaluate the effectiveness of a school gardening intervention on children’s fruit and vegetable consumption. One of the main limitations of previous studies in this area was their study design and the use of convenience sampling. 41–43 The strength of the current study is that schools were randomised to either one of the intervention groups or the comparison group; therefore, there was no possibility of introducing selection bias into the study. One of the fundamental problems with previous research in this area is that schools were selected based on their having or not having a garden; those without a garden were used as control schools. 14,44 Other biases included constraints of the school district and characteristics such as pupil numbers,46 self-selection and teachers being given the option to choose their condition group. 47,48,126
The sample size at baseline for both trials was lower than anticipated, with the response rate at follow-up for both trials combined being 62%. This reduced the average group size to approximately 388, 94 children fewer than the proposed sample size of 482, reducing the power of the study. Small sample sizes can lead to an underestimation of the SEs and affect the sensitivity of the tests used to determine the statistical differences between the groups. The sensitivity analysis (baseline brought forward) was conducted to explore whether or not the reduced sample size had an effect on the primary outcome. The results were very similar to those of the main analysis, suggesting that the reduced sample size did not affect the primary analysis of the trial. Furthermore, these trials are the largest trials to evaluate school gardening to date. Some of the studies in this area had a very small sample size, with some only involving a few schools or one school implementing the intervention. 41,42,44,125,128,129 Furthermore, the current trials involved a highly diverse population in terms of ethnicity and socioeconomic groups.
The dietary data were collected using a validated 24-hour food tick list (CADET) for children aged 3–11 years. The strength of the CADET diary is that it uses age- and gender-specific food portion sizes to calculate food and nutrient intake. A 1-day tick list is an economically effective way of gathering nutrient information from children. However, the disadvantage of using a 24-hour food frequency questionnaire is that it uses pre-allocated portion sizes for each item in CADET, based on average weighed intakes from UK children. 62 A 1-day tick list may not reflect true nutrient intake in the longer term. This study attempted to improve the quality of the data by providing parents and children with an instruction DVD to help explain how to complete the CADET home food diary. In this study, the trained fieldworkers also collected and reviewed all home food diaries. This was for two reasons: to reduce errors in the data collected to make sure that children did consume everything ticked on the diary, but also to obtain a retrospective recall for children who did not return the home food diary. The CADET diary does avoid these issues with child self-reported food intake, and is less of a burden on participants than the most commonly used alternative, a weighed 4-day food diary.
All the results were analysed using a robust statistical methodology, multilevel analysis. The benefit of this technique is that the means and CIs for the different foods and nutrients will be more accurate, as the children within a school are more similar to each other in terms of their food consumption, with less variability within the sample, compared with a random sample from the whole population. 86,124 The primary outcome measuring children’s fruit and vegetable consumption, using multilevel regression analysis, was originally intended to explore differences in follow-up intake, adjusting for baseline intake. However, owing to the negative distribution of the residuals for follow-up fruit and vegetable intake, a change score was calculated by subtracting baseline fruit and vegetable intake from follow-up intake. For both trials there was no imbalance between intervention and comparison groups for baseline intake of fruit and vegetables, suggesting that this was an appropriate methodology. 130
Conclusion
This is the first clustered RCT to explore whether or not a gardening intervention can increase children’s fruit and vegetable intake. The results showed that there was no change in children’s fruit and vegetable intake after receiving either the RHS-led or the teacher-led intervention.
Summary
This chapter has explored the primary outcome for both trials, asking ‘Can the RHS Campaign for School Gardening lead to increases in vegetable and fruit intake in children aged 8–9 years?’ It is evident that children’s fruit and vegetable intake did not significantly increase after participating in either the RHS- or teacher-led interventions. For both trials, the teacher-led intervention group had, on average, a higher mean change in fruit and vegetable intake compared with the RHS-led or comparison group. Further chapters will explore the adherence to the different interventions (RHS-led and teacher-led) and identify how the different types of interventions implemented affected the primary outcome of children’s fruit and vegetable intake.
Secondary outcomes were also explored: the effect of the RHS campaign on intake of key nutrients (fat, carbohydrate, protein, vitamin C, carotene, iron, sodium and folate), and whether or not there was an interaction effect between the intervention and gender. The only significant difference found in the secondary outcomes was for vitamin C intake in trial 1. Once the adjustments were made, there was a 13.0-mg-per-day difference between the RHS- and teacher-led groups, with the teacher-led group having a significantly higher intake of vitamin C.
Chapter 6 Impact of a school gardening intervention on children’s knowledge of and attitudes towards fruit and vegetables
Introduction
The psychological theory behind school gardens is based on the SCT. 131 The SCT is based on the assumption that to successfully change a person’s behaviour requires changing their knowledge, values and beliefs. 132 SCT has been used to design several gardening interventions. 41,43,44,46,47,133 Personal factors such as nutrition knowledge, food preferences (including willingness to taste), attitudes towards food and self-efficacy in eating and preparing food have already been associated with increased fruit and vegetable consumption in children and adolescents in non-gardening research. 134 Overall, gardening interventions have been associated with an increase in children’s nutrition knowledge in the majority of the studies which assessed this,42,44–47,73,126,133 though not in all. 41,43
In order to assess children’s knowledge and attitudes towards fruit and vegetables, a short questionnaire was developed and administered at baseline before and after the RHS interventions were implemented in the two trials. The aim of this chapter was to compare the effects of teacher-led gardening with those of the RHS-led school gardening intervention and no intervention at all, in terms of impact on children’s knowledge of and attitudes towards fruit and vegetables.
Method
All schools were provided with two copies of the child questionnaire for each child to complete individually, at baseline and then at follow-up after two growing seasons.
Fruit and vegetable knowledge
To assess knowledge of the 5 A DAY fruit and vegetable campaign, children were asked to circle on the child questionnaire a number between 1 and 8 in answer to the question ‘How many servings of fruit and vegetables do you think you should eat every day to stay healthy?’ To test children’s ability to recognise different fruit and vegetables, they were asked to draw a line from the names of 12 fruits and 16 vegetables to a colour photo of each item. Apple was provided as an example. All the fruits were listed and pictured on one page; these were raspberries, blackberries, pears, blueberries, plums, bananas, grapes, orange, pineapple, nectarine, watermelon and kiwi fruit. The following vegetables were listed on another page: courgettes, spinach, French beans, parsley, lettuces, parsnips, radish, sweetcorn, carrots, leeks, spring onions, broccoli, peppers, cucumber, tomatoes and garlic (see Appendix 1). For each item, correct responses were coded 1 and incorrect responses were coded 0.
Attitudes towards fruit and vegetables
To assess attitudes towards fruit and vegetables, the children were asked to circle responses indicating whether they agreed a lot, agreed a little, disagreed a little or disagreed a lot with 10 questions (Table 37), such as ‘I enjoy eating fruit’ or ‘I like trying new fruit’, which relate to perceived barriers to consumption. Self-efficacy was assessed using ‘I try to eat lots of fruit’ and ‘I’m good at preparing fruit and vegetables’. Perceived social influences and availability in the home environment were evaluated with the questions ‘My family encourages me to eat fruit and vegetables’ and ‘There’s usually lots of fruit and vegetables to eat at home’.
Attitudes and perceptions | Percentage of children who agreeda | Odds of agreeing (95% CI) at follow-up using MLM to compare interventions | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
RHS-led (n = 366) | Teacher-led (n = 394) | Unadjusted | Adjusted for baseline | Additional adjustmentb | |||
Baseline | Follow-up | Baseline | Follow-up | ||||
I enjoy eating fruit | 94.5 | 91.8 | 96.4 | 96.2 | 0.4 (0.2 to 1.0) | 0.5 (0.2 to 1.1) | 0.4 (0.1 to 1.0) |
I like trying new fruits | 78.0 | 76.3 | 83.3 | 86.6 | 0.5 (0.2 to 0.9) | 0.5 (0.2 to 0.9) | 0.5 (0.2 to 0.9) |
I try to eat lots of fruit | 83.0 | 81.3 | 86.7 | 90.1 | 0.4 (0.2 to 0.8) | 0.4 (0.2 to 0.8) | 0.4 (0.2 to 0.9) |
I enjoy eating vegetables | 65.6 | 64.7 | 66.9 | 65.9 | 1.0 (0.5 to 1.8) | 1.0 (0.5 to 1.9) | 1.1 (0.6 to 1.9) |
I like trying new vegetables | 58.9 | 58.0 | 61.0 | 60.0 | 0.9 (0.6 to 1.4) | 0.9 (0.6 to 1.4) | 1.0 (0.7 to 1.5) |
I try to eat lots of vegetables | 64.6 | 70.9 | 66.7 | 69.6 | 1.1 (0.6 to 1.9) | 1.1 (0.7 to 1.8) | 1.1 (0.7 to 1.7) |
Eating fruit and vegetables every day keeps me healthy | 93.5 | 94.1 | 94.1 | 97.2 | 0.5 (0.2 to 1.8) | 0.5 (0.1 to 1.7) | 0.6 (0.2 to 1.6) |
There’s usually lots of fruit and vegetables to eat at home | 89.2 | 89.8 | 87.6 | 94.1 | 0.5 (0.2 to 1.0) | 0.5 (0.2 to 1.0) | 0.4 (0.2 to 0.9) |
I’m good at preparing fruit and vegetablesc | 71.8 | 74.7 | 81.3 | 83.6 | 0.6 (0.3 to 0.9) | 0.6 (0.3 to 1.4) | 0.6 (0.3 to 1.1) |
My family encourages me to eat fruit and vegetables | 87.1 | 90.7 | 88.3 | 93.7 | 0.7 (0.3 to 1.4) | 0.7 (0.3 to 1.5) | 0.7 (0.3 to 1.5) |
Percentage who knew that five fruit and vegetables per day are needed to stay healthy | 76.2 | 79.0 | 72.7 | 79.0 | 0.9 (0.4 to 1.1) | 0.8 (0.6 to 1.5) | 0.9 (0.4 to 1.6) |
Percentage who had tasted their own fruit and vegetables at follow-up | 62.3 | 62.1 | 52.4 | 67.8 | 0.8 (0.4 to 1.2) | – | 0.8 (0.5 to 1.4) |
Gardening experience
To determine gardening experience, the children were asked to circle ‘yes’ or ‘no’ in answer to ‘We grow fruit and vegetables in our garden or allotment’. They were then asked ‘What fruit or vegetables have you grown?’. For each child the number of different types of fruit and vegetables listed were coded as two separate variables. Finally, they were asked ‘Have you tasted any fruit or vegetables from your garden or allotment?’ (‘yes’ or ‘no’), and ‘What fruit or vegetables have you tasted?’. Each child’s list of tasted items was compared with his or her list of own-grown fruit and vegetables, and recorded for analysis as ‘none’, ’some’ or ‘all fruit and vegetables grown’.
Statistical analysis
Differences between intervention groups for descriptive variables were analysed using chi-squared tests for categorical variables and t-tests for continuous variables.
Multilevel mixed-effects logistic regression analyses were used to determine whether or not there were significant differences between intervention groups at follow-up, relating to knowledge of the five fruit and vegetables a day needed to remain healthy. This method was also used to analyse differences relating to the percentage of children who agreed (a little or a lot) and the percentage of those who disagreed (a little or a lot) with the attitude statements. Odds ratios (ORs) were presented unadjusted, and also adjusted for baseline answers. Further analysis on > 90% of the children also adjusted for gender, ethnicity and IMDS. In these mixed-effects analyses, the fixed effects variable was the gardening intervention and the random effects variable was the school. The percentages of children who correctly identified 5 A DAY requirements and the individual fruit and vegetables, and the percentage who agreed with the attitude statements were also tabulated. Multilevel mixed-effects logistic regression models were also used to compare, between interventions, the percentage of children able to identify individual fruit and vegetables.
The change from baseline to follow-up in the total numbers of fruit and vegetables recognised was also calculated for each qualifying child, and compared between interventions for both trials using independent samples t-tests. p-values from multilevel mixed-effects regression analysis, adjusted for gender, ethnicity and IMDS, were also tabulated. These methods were used to assess the change between baseline and follow-up in the number of types of fruit or vegetables children listed as own-grown.
Multilevel mixed-effects regression analysis was used to determine whether or not there was an association between the change in knowledge of fruit and vegetables and the change in actual mean fruit and vegetable intake derived from the school and home food diaries. Analyses were presented unadjusted and adjusted for gender, ethnicity and IMDS. Only children who completed both the baseline and the follow-up questions of the appropriate section of the child questionnaire were included in these analyses. Statistical analyses was performed using Stata version 12. p-values of < 0.05 were taken to represent statistical significance for all analyses, except that relating to the recognition of individual fruit and vegetables, where p-values of < 0.01 were taken as statistically significant because of multiple testing.
Results
Response rate
In trial 1, 404 children (69%) from the teacher-led group and 373 (70%) from the RHS-led intervention attempted parts of both the baseline and follow-up child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire. In trial 2, 559 children (77%) from the teacher-led intervention and 541 (71%) from the control group attempted this. Not all of these children completed every section of the questionnaire. The numbers of children with completed dietary data were 329 (56%) from the teacher-led group and 323 (61%) from the RHS-led group in trial 1, and 500 (69%) from the teacher-led group and 431 (57%) from the comparison group in trial 2.
Attitudes towards fruit and vegetables
In relation to children’s attitudes towards and perceptions about fruit and vegetables, > 90% of the children from the two trials at both baseline and follow-up agreed that eating vegetables every day kept them healthy and that their parents encouraged them to eat these. Over 90% of the children at both baseline and follow-up agreed that they enjoyed eating fruit, whereas 60–70% agreed that they enjoyed eating vegetables, and only 50–60% agreed that they liked trying new vegetables (see Table 37). In trial 2 (Table 38), children in the gardening intervention group were significantly more likely to agree that they enjoyed eating vegetables at follow-up compared with the control group (69.5% vs. 61.7%), even after adjusting for baseline answers (OR = 1.3, 95% CI 1.0 to 1.8); however, this was not significant after adjusting for gender, ethnicity or IMDS (OR = 1.2, 95% CI 0.9 to 1.6, p = 0.1). There were no other significant differences in trial 2 for this section of the questionnaire, and there were no significant differences relating to vegetables in trial 1 with regards to answers at follow-up. However, children in the RHS-led group in trial 1 were significantly less likely to agree that they tried to eat lots of fruit or liked to try new fruit than those in the teacher-led group, even after baseline adjustments (OR = 0.4, 95% CI 0.2 to 0.8, p = 0.009 and OR = 0.5, 95% CI 0.2 to 0.9, p = 0.05 respectively). In addition, after further adjustment for sociodemographic factors (including deprivation score), children in the RHS-led group were significantly less likely than those in the teacher-led group to agree that there were lots of fruit and vegetables to eat at home (OR = 0.4, 95% CI 0.2 to 0.9, p = 0. 02).
Attitudes and perceptions | Percentage of children who agreeda | Odds of agreeing at follow-up (95% CI) using MLM to compare interventions | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Teacher-led (n = 537) | Control group (n = 498) | Unadjusted | Adjusted for baseline | Additional adjustmentb | |||
Baseline | Follow-up | Baseline | Follow-up | ||||
I enjoy eating fruit | 96.7 | 97.6 | 96.8 | 97.0 | 1.2 (0.5 to 2.8) | 1.1 (0.5 to 2.7) | 1.1 (0.4 to 2.9) |
I like trying new fruits | 86.0 | 84.0 | 84.5 | 80.4 | 1.2 (0.8 to 1.9) | 1.2 (0.8 to 1.9) | 1.2 (0.7 to 1.9) |
I try to eat lots of fruitc | 87.2 | 88.2 | 82.7 | 85.8 | 1.2 (0.7 to 1.8) | 1.1 (0.7 to 1.8) | 1.0 (0.6 to 1.6) |
I enjoy eating vegetables | 68.8 | 69.5 | 64.2 | 61.7 | 1.4 (1.0 to 1.8) | 1.3 (1.0 to 1.8) | 1.2 (0.9 to 1.6) |
I like trying new vegetables | 62.8 | 59.5 | 60.5 | 56.9 | 1.1 (0.8 to 1.4) | 1.0 (0.8 to 1.4) | 0.9 (0.7 to 1.2) |
I try to eat lots of vegetablesc | 72.8 | 75.5 | 66.7 | 68.6 | 1.4 (0.9 to 2.0) | 1.3 (0.9 to 1.9) | 1.2 (0.8 to 1.8) |
Eating fruit and vegetables every day keeps me healthy | 94.9 | 97.0 | 96.2 | 96.4 | 1.2 (0.5 to 2.7) | 1.2 (0.5 to 2.7) | 1.2 (0.5 to 2.8) |
There’s usually lots of fruit and vegetables to eat at home | 89.6 | 92.8 | 88.9 | 89.5 | 1.5 (0.9 to 2.3) | 1.5 (0.9 to 2.3) | 1.5 (0.9 to 2.5) |
I’m good at preparing fruit and vegetables | 79.3 | 78.1 | 77.9 | 79.3 | 0.9 (0.6 to 1.2) | 0.9 (0.6 to 1.2) | 0.8 (0.6 to 1.1) |
My family encourages me to eat fruit and vegetables | 89.9 | 92.8 | 87.7 | 91.9 | 1.1 (0.7 to 1.7) | 1.1 (0.6 to 1.7) | 0.9 (0.5 to 1.6) |
Percentage who knew that five fruit and vegetables per day are needed to stay healthyc | 73.6 | 79.1 | 67.3 | 67.5 | 1.8 (1.2 to 2.8) | 1.7 (1.1 to 2.6) | 1.7 (1.1 to 2.5) |
Percentage who had tasted their own fruit and vegetables at follow-up | 60.1 | 66.4 | 56.0 | 58.1 | 1.4 (0.8 to 2.4) | 1.4 (0.8 to 2.4) | 1.4 (0.8 to 2.4) |
At baseline a high proportion of children (> 67%) knew that five servings of fruit and vegetables should be eaten every day to stay healthy. Of the children who answered this question at both baseline and follow-up, there was no significant difference in the proportion giving correct answers at follow-up between the RHS- and teacher-led groups in trial 1 (79% vs. 79%). However, there was a significant difference between the intervention groups in trial 2 at follow-up, with the teacher-led group giving more correct answers than the comparison group (79% vs. 68%). From the multilevel logistic regression analyses, a significant difference remained (OR = 1.7, 95% CI 1.1 to 2.6, p = 0.006) after adjusting for baseline answers (which were significantly different between groups) and also after further adjustment for sociodemographic factors (OR = 1.7, 95% CI 1.1 to 2.5, p = 0.004).
Additionally, there was no evidence that the school gardening interventions significantly increased the likelihood of children tasting their own-grown fruit and vegetables.
The children’s ability to recognise different fruit was already very good at baseline. Table 39 reports the mean number of fruit and vegetables recognised at baseline and follow-up for both trials. It is evident when comparing the change in total fruit recognised from baseline to follow-up that there was no significant difference between intervention groups, in either trial 1 or trial 2, in the unadjusted independent t-test analyses or after adjustment for sociodemographic variables in multilevel analyses. Similarly, there was no significant difference in the change in total vegetables recognised between intervention groups in trial 2. However, in trial 1 there was a significantly larger increase in the number of different vegetables recognised from baseline to follow-up for the RHS-led group compared with the teacher-led group (a mean increase of 2.44 vs. 1.65 out of a total of 16 vegetables). This was statistically significant (p = 0.03) in multilevel analysis after additionally adjusting for sociodemographic factors.
Trial and intervention | n | Baseline mean (95% CI) | Follow-up mean (95% CI) | Mean change (95% CI) | p a | p b |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Trial 1 | ||||||
Fruit | ||||||
RHS-led | 373 | 10.6 (10.5 to 10.8) | 11.0 (10.9 to 11.2) | 0.37 (0.16 to 0.58) | 0.7 | 0.9 |
Teacher-led | 404 | 10.9 (10.8 to 11.1)c | 11.2 (11.1 to 11.4) | 0.31 (0.14 to 0.48) | ||
Vegetables | ||||||
RHS-led | 369 | 10.4 (10.1 to 10.7) | 12.9 (12.6 to 13.1) | 2.44 (2.01 to 2.83) | 0.002 | 0.03 |
Teacher-led | 404 | 11.3 (10.9 to 11.6)c | 12.9 (12.6 to 13.2) | 1.65 (1.34 to 1.98) | ||
Total fruit and vegetables | ||||||
RHS-led | 372 | 20.9 (20.5 to 21.4) | 23.9 (23.5 to 24.2) | 2.79 (2.32 to 3.26) | 0.007 | 0.08 |
Teacher-led | 404 | 22.1 (21.8 to 22.6) | 24.2 (23.8 to 24.5) | 1.96 (1.59 to 2.34) | ||
Trial 2 | ||||||
Fruit | ||||||
Teacher-led | 556 | 10.5 (10.3 to 10.6) | 11.0 (10.8 to 11.1) | 0.51 (0.35 to 0.67) | 0.2 | 0.3 |
Control | 535 | 10.4 (10.3 to 10.6) | 11.1 (11.0 to 11.2) | 0.67 (0.49 to 0.85) | ||
Vegetables | ||||||
Teacher-led | 552 | 10.8 (10.5 to 11.0) | 12.4 (12.1 to 12.7) | 1.65 (1.36 to 1.95) | 0.3 | 0.6 |
Control | 532 | 10.7 (10.4 to 11.0) | 12.1 (11.9 to 12.4) | 1.45 (1.17 to 1.72) | ||
Total fruit and vegetables | ||||||
Teacher-led | 558 | 21.1 (20.7 to 21.5) | 23.3 (23.0 to 23.7) | 2.15 (1.78 to 2.51) | 0.8 | 0.9 |
Control | 536 | 21.0 (20.7 to 21.4) | 23.1 (22.8 to 23.5) | 2.10 (1.74 to 2.45) |
Similarly, in trial 1 there was a significantly larger increase in the total number of fruits and vegetables recognised from baseline to follow-up for the RHS-led group compared with the teacher-led group (p = 0.007 in the t-test), but this was not significant after adjusting for sociodemographic variables in multilevel models.
In both trials (Tables 40 and 41; Figures 20–23), ≥ 80% of children were able to identify each type of fruit on the questionnaire, apart from blackberries, blueberries, plums and nectarines, which were identified by only ≥ 64% of children. Over 90% of the children could identify pears, bananas, grapes, oranges, pineapples and watermelons. In contrast, the ability to recognise vegetables was more varied, with 90% of children recognising sweetcorn, carrots, peppers and tomatoes, but only 50% identifying spinach, parsley, leeks and spring onions.
Fruit or vegetable | Percentage of children who recognised the item | Percentage-point difference at follow-up (RHS-led minus teacher-led) | Difference in change between baseline and follow-up (%) | Chi-squared test: p-value for difference at follow-up | p-value for difference at follow-up using MLM logistic regression | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
RHS-led intervention (n = 372) | Teacher-led intervention (n = 404) | ||||||||
Baseline | Follow-up | Baseline | Follow-up | Adjusted for baselinea | Further adjustedb | ||||
Raspberries | 84.7 | 90.9 | 89.9 | 94.8 | –3.9 | 1.2 | 0.3 | 0.08 | 0.03 |
Blackberries | 82.5 | 86.8 | 83.9 | 90.3 | –3.5 | –2.0 | 0.1 | 0.5 | 0.6 |
Pears | 93.8 | 96.8 | 96.5 | 97.8 | –1.0 | 1.7 | 0.4 | 0.4 | 0.5 |
Blueberries | 76.9 | 81.7 | 82.2 | 88.1 | –6.4 | –1.1 | 0.01 | 0.4 | 0.7 |
Plums | 80.4 | 82.0 | 82.6 | 84.2 | –2.2 | 0.1 | 0.4 | 0.6 | 0.7 |
Bananasc,d | 96.8 | 98.9 | 99.5 | 99.0 | –0.1 | 2.6 | 0.9 | – | – |
Grapes | 91.9 | 96.0 | 94.8 | 97.5 | –1.6 | 1.3 | 0.2 | 0.3 | 0.5 |
Oranges | 96.5 | 96.8 | 97.5 | 96.5 | 0.2 | 1.3 | 0.9 | 0.8 | 1.0 |
Pineapples | 96.5 | 96.8 | 96.0 | 98.0 | –1.2 | –1.7 | 0.3 | 0.3 | 0.5 |
Nectarines | 70.7 | 76.9 | 75.7 | 81.4 | –4.6 | 0.5 | 0.1 | 0.3 | 0.5 |
Watermelonsd | 98.7 | 99.5 | 98.8 | 98.5 | 0.9 | 1.1 | 0.2 | 0.3 | 0.3 |
Kiwifruit | 95.4 | 98.1 | 95.1 | 97.8 | 0.3 | 0.0 | 0.7 | 0.6 | 0.8 |
Courgettes | 46.7 | 72.0 | 55.3 | 74.8 | –2.9 | 5.7 | 0.4 | 0.9 | 0.7 |
Spinach | 39.7 | 63.9 | 43.4 | 62.8 | 1.1 | 4.8 | 0.8 | 0.5 | 0.9 |
French beans | 71.7 | 83.7 | 76.9 | 83.4 | 0.3 | 5.5 | 0.9 | 0.6 | 0.8 |
Parsleyc | 27.5 | 52.0 | 36.2 | 49.1 | 2.9 | 11.7 | 0.4 | 0.2 | 0.2 |
Lettuces | 78.5 | 90.8 | 82.6 | 91.3 | –0.6 | 3.5 | 0.8 | 0.9 | 1.0 |
Parsnipsc | 38.9 | 62.0 | 51.9 | 65.0 | –3.1 | 9.9 | 0.4 | 0.9 | 1.0 |
Radishes | 52.7 | 74.7 | 61.0 | 75.7 | –1.0 | 7.3 | 0.7 | 0.7 | 0.8 |
Sweetcornd | 96.2 | 98.4 | 98.0 | 99.5 | –1.1 | 0.7 | 0.1 | 0.1 | – |
Carrotsd | 94.6 | 99.2 | 97.5 | 98.8 | 0.4 | 3.4 | 0.6 | 0.5 | 0.3 |
Leeks | 38.0 | 56.5 | 41.7 | 62.3 | –5.8 | –2.1 | 0.1 | 0.4 | 0.3 |
Spring onions | 38.9 | 59.2 | 40.7 | 61.5 | –2.3 | –0.5 | 0.5 | 0.9 | 0.6 |
Broccolic | 88.6 | 96.2 | 94.8 | 94.0 | 2.2 | 8.4 | 0.2 | 0.2 | 0.2 |
Peppers | 90.2 | 97.3 | 95.0 | 97.3 | 0.0 | 4.8 | 1.0 | 0.8 | 0.9 |
Cucumbers | 73.1 | 89.1 | 80.7 | 89.6 | –0.5 | 7.1 | 0.8 | 0.9 | 0.6 |
Tomatoes | 97.0 | 98.1 | 98.3 | 98.2 | –0.2 | 1.1 | 0.9 | – | – |
Garlic | 68.2 | 93.2 | 71.6 | 88.1 | 5.1 | 8.5 | 0.02 | 0.03 | 0.04 |
Fruit or vegetable | Percentage of children who recognised the item | Percentage-point difference at follow-up (teacher-led minus control group) | Difference in change between baseline and follow-up (%) | Chi-squared test: p-value for difference at follow-up | p-value for difference at follow-up using MLM logistic regression | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Teacher-led intervention (n = 556) | Control group (n = 535) | ||||||||
Baseline | Follow-up | Baseline | Follow-up | Adjusted for baselinea | Further adjustedb | ||||
Raspberries | 83.2 | 92.6 | 85.8 | 92.2 | 0.5 | 3.0 | 0.8 | 0.7 | 0.9 |
Blackberries | 78.2 | 86.5 | 80.9 | 89.7 | –3.2 | –0.5 | 0.1 | 0.2 | 0.2 |
Pears | 95.3 | 98.0 | 94.2 | 97.2 | 0.8 | –0.3 | 0.4 | 0.4 | 0.4 |
Blueberries | 73.9 | 84.2 | 74.4 | 83.6 | 0.6 | 1.1 | 0.8 | 0.8 | 1.0 |
Plums | 74.5 | 79.0 | 74.4 | 83.6 | –4.6 | –4.7 | 0.05 | 0.08 | 0.06 |
Bananas | 98.6 | 98.7 | 97.6 | 97.8 | 1.0 | 0.0 | 0.2 | 0.2 | 0.2 |
Grapes | 93.5 | 96.4 | 91.2 | 96.6 | –0.2 | –2.5 | 0.8 | 0.8 | 0.4 |
Oranges | 96.2 | 96.4 | 95.9 | 96.1 | 0.3 | 0.0 | 0.8 | 0.8 | 1.0 |
Pineapples | 95.5 | 97.7 | 94.8 | 97.2 | 0.5 | –0.3 | 0.8 | 0.7 | 0.5 |
Nectarines | 64.9 | 71.4 | 64.5 | 78.5 | –7.1 | –7.5 | 0.007 | 0.006 | 0.001 |
Watermelons | 97.5 | 98.6 | 95.9 | 99.1 | –0.5 | –2.1 | 0.4 | 0.4 | 0.2 |
Kiwifruit | 94.2 | 96.9 | 92.9 | 97.9 | –1.0 | –2.3 | 0.3 | 0.4 | 0.3 |
Courgettes | 50.4 | 66.7 | 47.5 | 61.8 | 4.9 | 1.9 | 0.1 | 0.4 | 0.5 |
Spinach | 45.1 | 62.0 | 43.7 | 55.6 | 6.4 | 4.9 | 0.03 | 0.3 | 0.5 |
French beans | 74.3 | 84.2 | 69.9 | 81.9 | 2.4 | –2.0 | 0.3 | 0.8 | 0.7 |
Parsley | 31.5 | 46.4 | 30.4 | 44.4 | 2.0 | 0.9 | 0.5 | 0.7 | 0.9 |
Lettuces | 79.9 | 89.3 | 78.6 | 87.9 | 1.3 | 0.0 | 0.5 | 0.8 | 0.9 |
Parsnips | 48.0 | 57.6 | 44.4 | 56.1 | 1.5 | –2.1 | 0.6 | 1.0 | 0.8 |
Radishes | 53.1 | 70.8 | 50.3 | 64.3 | 6.5 | 3.7 | 0.02 | 0.2 | 0.2 |
Sweetcorn | 97.1 | 98.4 | 97.2 | 98.5 | –0.1 | –0.1 | 0.9 | 0.9 | 0.3 |
Carrots | 95.7 | 96.9 | 95.8 | 98.5 | –1.6 | –1.4 | 0.09 | 0.1 | 0.5 |
Leeks | 39.0 | 53.1 | 36.3 | 43.9 | 9.1 | 6.5 | 0.003 | 0.08 | 0.1 |
Spring onions | 40.4 | 56.0 | 43.1 | 57.5 | –1.5 | 1.2 | 0.6 | 0.7 | 0.6 |
Broccoli | 89.5 | 93.3 | 91.3 | 94.7 | –1.4 | 0.4 | 0.3 | 0.4 | 1.0 |
Peppers | 90.8 | 94.9 | 92.6 | 96.2 | –1.3 | 0.6 | 0.3 | 0.4 | 0.4 |
Cucumbers | 76.1 | 86.1 | 79.2 | 89.0 | –3.0 | 0.1 | 0.1 | 0.2 | 0.4 |
Tomatoes | 96.2 | 98.2 | 96.8 | 96.8 | 1.4 | 2.0 | 0.1 | – | – |
Garlic | 69.0 | 87.9 | 71.3 | 86.2 | 1.7 | 3.9 | 0.4 | 0.5 | 0.6 |
Nevertheless, about 30% of children in trial 1 and > 20% of children in trial 2 identified these last four vegetables correctly for the first time at follow-up, after the gardening intervention. The figures, however, show that a fair proportion of children could not identify these and other items (such as blackberries, blueberries, plums and nectarines) at follow-up after previously identifying them correctly at baseline, as some of the answers were probably guesses.
In trial 1 there were no differences at follow-up between RHS-led and teacher-led interventions which were significant at p < 0.01. In trial 2, significant differences at follow-up between the teacher-led intervention and the control group were found only in relation to nectarines in both chi-squared tests and multilevel models adjusting for baseline, and additionally for sociodemographic variables (at p < 0.001). Children in the control group were more likely to identify nectarines than those in the teacher-led intervention. Children in the teacher-led intervention, however, were significantly more likely to be able to identify leeks at follow-up than those in the control group, but this was not significant after baseline adjustments in multilevel models.
Using multilevel mixed-effects regression analysis, there was no significant evidence in any of the gardening groups of an association between the change in fruit or vegetables, or total fruit and vegetables, identified from baseline to follow-up, and the change in actual intake of fruit or vegetables derived from the school and home food diaries. Although the results for trial 1 showed decreases in fruit intake these were not statistically significant. Conversely, point estimates for trial 2 indicated an increase in intake with increased recognition for vegetables in the teacher-led gardening group, though again this was not statistically significant (Table 42).
Trial and intervention | n | Unadjusted mean change in intake (g) (95% CI) | Adjusted mean change in intake (g) (95% CI)a | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Trial 1 | ||||
Fruit | ||||
RHS-led | 295 | –0.05 (–11.3 to 11.2) | –1.59 (–13.3 to 10.2) | 0.8 |
Teacher-led | 317 | –4.71 (–17.7 to 8.25) | –3.62 (–16.3 to 9.03) | 0.6 |
Vegetables | ||||
RHS-led | 293 | 0.43 (–2.69 to 4.55) | –0.29 (–3.07 to 3.01) | 1.0 |
Teacher-led | 312 | 1.35 (–2.27 to 4.97) | 1.36 (–2.23 to 4.95) | 0.5 |
Fruit and vegetables | ||||
RHS-led | 292 | 0.71 (–4.98 to 6.39) | 0.03 (–5.71 to 5.78) | 0.8 |
Teacher-led | 312 | –1.52 (–8.45 to 5.41) | –1.59 (–8.43 to 5.26) | 0.7 |
Trial 2 | ||||
Fruit | ||||
Teacher-led | 467 | –3.54 (–13.7 to 6.55) | –3.71 (–13.7 to 6.26) | 0.5 |
Control | 405 | –1.24 (–10.7 to 8.18) | –2.19 (–11.7 to 7.30) | 0.7 |
Vegetables | ||||
Teacher-led | 460 | 1.68 (–1.16 to 4.53) | 1.77 (–1.08 to 4.61) | 0.2 |
Control | 403 | –2.13 (–5.90 to 1.65) | –1.68 (–5.46 to 2.09) | 0.4 |
Fruit and vegetables | ||||
Teacher-led | 459 | –0.91 (–6.16 to 4.34) | –0.87 (–6.05 to 4.32) | 0.7 |
Control | 401 | 0.67 (–5.32 to 6.65) | 0.82 (–5.21 to 6.84) | 0.8 |
There was no significant difference between the RHS- and teacher-led groups in trial 1 in the change between baseline and follow-up for the number of types of fruit or vegetables children listed as own grown. However, in trial 2 there was a significant increase in the number of types of own-grown fruit listed by the teacher-led group compared with the control group (mean = 0.3, 95% CI 0 to 0.6), but a significant decrease in the number of types of vegetables listed. After adjusting for gender, ethnicity, IMDS and baseline, however, these differences were no longer significant (Table 43).
Trial and intervention | n | Baseline mean (95% CI) | Follow-up mean (95% CI) | Mean change (95% CI) | p a | p b |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Trial 1 | ||||||
Fruit | ||||||
RHS-led | 77 | 1.9 (1.7 to 2.3) | 1.8 (1.6 to 2.1) | –0.1 (–0.5 to 0.2) | 0.3 | 0.9 |
Teacher-led | 105 | 2.0 (1.8 to 2.3) | 2.2 (1.9 to 2.5) | 0.1 (–0.2 to 0.4) | ||
Vegetables | ||||||
RHS-led | 120 | 2.4 (2.1 to 2.6) | 2.6 (2.3 to 2.9) | 0.3 (–0.1 to 0.6) | 0.1 | 0.07 |
Teacher-led | 169 | 2.7 (2.4 to 3.0) | 2.6 (2.2 to 2.9) | –0.1 (–0.5 to 0.2) | ||
Trial 2 | ||||||
Fruit | ||||||
Teacher-led | 126 | 1.9 (1.6 to 2.1) | 2.15 (1.9 to 2.4) | 0.3 (0.0 to 0.6) | 0.05 | 0.2 |
Control | 121 | 2.1 (1.8 to 2.3) | 1.91 (1.7 to 2.1) | –0.1 (–0.5 to 0.2) | ||
Vegetables | ||||||
Teacher-led | 142 | 2.5 (2.2 to 2.7) | 2.4 (2.2 to 2.6) | –0.1 (–0.3 to 0.2) | 0.005 | 0.02 |
Control | 221 | 2.0 (1.8 to 2.2) | 2.5 (2.2 to 2.9) | 0.5 (0.2 to 0.9) |
Using multilevel mixed-effects regression analysis there was no significant evidence in any of the gardening groups of an association between the change in fruit or vegetable intake, and growing fruit and vegetables at home or tasting fruit and vegetables grown at home. This analysis is presented in Table 44 with the unadjusted and adjusted models.
Trial | n | Unadjusted mean change (g) (95% CI) | Adjusted mean change (g) (95% CI) | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
Do you grow your own fruit and vegetables? | ||||
Trial 1 | 608 | 20 (–20 to 61) | 21 (–10 to 74) | 0.1 |
Trial 2 | 881 | 2 (–40 to 34) | 3 (–34 to 41) | 0.8 |
Have you tasted the fruit and vegetables you have grown? | ||||
Trial 1 | 608 | –3 (–44 to 37) | 13 (–30 to 57) | 0.7 |
Trial 2 | 881 | 22 (–8 to 71) | 22 (–8 to 71) | 0.1 |
Discussion
The results from the two RCTs provide very limited evidence that gardening interventions in schools increase children’s knowledge and awareness of, or attitudes towards, eating fruit and vegetables.
Knowledge
In trial 1, the RHS-led gardening group was associated with an increase in the total number of different vegetables recognised; however, this difference was not significant after adjustment for baseline measurement (which was significantly different between interventions). In addition, compared with the teacher-led group, the RHS intervention was associated with negative effects. On average, children allocated to the RHS-led group were likely to be able to identify significantly more vegetables after the intervention than the teacher-led group; however, this may be explained by the fact that there was significantly more scope for improvement from baseline in the RHS-led intervention group. Furthermore, there were no significant increases in the ability to identify individual vegetables. Moreover, the increase in total vegetable recognition was not associated with an increase in vegetable intake.
In trial 2, there were a few significant increases that remained after adjustment for sociodemographic variables in the teacher-led school gardening intervention compared with the comparison group, which did not receive any assistance or support with gardening activities in school. The teacher-led children were more likely to have an increased awareness of the 5 A DAY recommendations for staying healthy, and more likely to recognise nectarines (though no other fruit or vegetables) and to report a decrease in own-grown fruit compared with the comparison group. Additionally, there was no evidence in any of the gardening intervention groups that, on average, an increase in the number of fruit and vegetables recognised was associated with an actual increase in consumption of fruit and vegetables.
Contrary to the results of the current trials, previous US and Australian studies which tested for the identification of individual vegetables found significant increases in the ability to identify them in the gardening interventions compared with controls, after taking into account pre-test scores. 47,126,133 However, in contrast to the current two trials, these studies used real vegetables and tested only a small number of items (five to six), as opposed to the 16 photos of vegetables used in this study. Furthermore, studies that identified successful change in children’s nutrition knowledge combined health, science or nutrition education alongside the gardening component of their intervention studies. In our trials, the RHS- and teacher-led interventions focused solely on gardening education. This might explain the lack of significant findings in these trials. There were two previous studies that also found no significant change in children’s knowledge after implementing a gardening intervention; however, one did not include a control group and was a relatively small study consisting of 56 children,42 and the other44 was conducted with younger children than those in this sample (Grade 1). One previous Australian study73 used a larger number of pictures of fruit and vegetables to explore children’s knowledge (31 in total) and found a significant difference between pre- and post-identification scores; however, the historical control design was a weakness of the study. These previous studies involved only 320 or fewer children from one or two schools, compared with the 1867 children from 52 schools who took part in the pre- and post-fruit and vegetable identification tests in the current two studies. The majority of previous studies also involved older children, although they would have been more likely to produce a knowledge ceiling effect than the 8-year-olds in the current two studies.
Despite there being a greater increase in awareness of 5 A DAY in the teacher-led gardening intervention group compared with the comparison group, there were no significant differences in awareness among these children that eating fruit and vegetables kept them healthy. Other previous gardening intervention studies did not report awareness of 5 A DAY separately, although this question was included in the ‘Health and nutrition from the garden’ questionnaire135 developed for children and used in some of the existing studies. 41–43 A previous study also found no evidence that gardening interventions were associated with children being aware that eating fruit and vegetables kept them healthy. 73
Attitudes towards fruit and vegetables
Those in the RHS-led group appeared less willing to try to eat lots of fruit or to try new fruits than the teacher-led group, even after adjusting for baseline responses. Previous studies43,73 have also reported a perceived barrier to eating fruit and vegetables, finding that the gardening intervention group did not like trying new fruits compared with controls. 73 This result, however, was not found for trial 2. In trial 2, children in the gardening intervention group were significantly more likely to agree that they enjoyed eating vegetables at follow-up compared with the control group, even after adjusting for baseline answers; however, this difference became non-significant after adjusting for gender, ethnicity or IMDS. It is possible that the additional exposure to gardening in the RHS-led intervention group may make the children more certain of their dislikes, as this additional exposure may produce greater contemplation of fruit and vegetables. 73
In other studies, different approaches have been used to measure willingness to try new fruit and vegetables. In taste tests, gardening interventions were associated with an increased willingness to taste a small number of fruit and vegetables in kindergarten or first graders44,45 in some studies, but not in older children,46,133 though gardening was associated with an increased taste rating in older children in other studies. 47,126 Questionnaire assessment of preference/willingness to taste a larger list of fruits and vegetables showed that gardening interventions were associated with a preference for vegetables in some studies,133,136,137 but not associated with fruit and vegetable preferences in other studies. 41,42,44
In both the current trials there was no evidence of differences before or after adjustment for baseline answers in self-efficacy, specifically in the perceived ability to prepare fruit and vegetables. Older children in the intervention group were less confident than controls, but there were no significant differences between intervention groups in younger children. 73 The current research provides very limited evidence that gardening interventions in schools increase factors which may mediate behaviour change in consumption of fruit and vegetables based on the principles of SCT.
Limitations and strengths
There are some limitations. Despite randomisation of a large number of London schools there were some significant differences between intervention groups, not only those relating to baseline recognition and intake of fruit and vegetables. A large number of children from schools with children who spoke English as a second language could have resulted in many participants misunderstanding how to complete the questionnaires and could be a limitation of the study. Children for whom English is an additional language are less likely to know the names of less common fruits and vegetables, so differences between groups may result from different language acquisition. A large percentage of the children in the study (≈ 30%) did not attempt the child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire at both time points; therefore, the results are potentially subject to response bias, i.e. bias relating to self-selection. Finally, it is possible that some of the inconsistencies in the results are spurious in nature and are due to multiple testing.
Another limitation of measuring children’s knowledge is that, naturally, children do guess if they do not know the right answer. There are very few validated tools to explore nutrition knowledge in children. A design fault of the current knowledge questionnaire was that it did not provide the children with the option of ‘don’t know’; this might have reduced the percentage of children guessing, and improved the questionnaire’s ability to accurately measure knowledge.
Compared with previous studies, strengths of this study include the large sample size, the use of schools as a random effect variable in multilevel models and the randomisation of schools to the different interventions or comparison group. It has greater methodological strengths than the two studies on which some of the questions relating to attitudes, self-efficacy and home environment were based,73,138 and adjustment was made for baseline responses and current controls rather than historical controls. Most previous studies had follow-up periods which were less than 1 year, some being 16 weeks or less,43,47 whereas the follow-up period in this trial included two growing seasons and was 18 months in duration.
Conclusion
Compared with schools that do not garden with their children, some gardening activities in schools may increase some aspects of pupils’ awareness of, and willingness to grow and eat, fruit and vegetables. Inconsistencies found, suggest that more research should be done in this area in UK schools. One of the fundamental aspects of gardening interventions that have shown a change in children’s knowledge is that the interventions used contained a nutritional component combined with gardening. This would suggest that to improve children’s knowledge of fruit and vegetables, gardening alone is not enough.
Summary
This chapter has explored whether or not participating in the RHS- or teacher-led school gardening interventions improved or affected children’s knowledge of and/or attitudes towards fruit and vegetables. The results revealed very little evidence to support previous research that school gardening can improve children’s knowledge of and attitudes towards fruit and vegetables. Further analysis of the components involved in the intervention will be discussed in the next chapter. The results from these two trials indicate that the RHS-led gardening intervention in schools does not provide extra benefits over the teacher-led intervention.
Chapter 7 Process evaluation of a randomised controlled trial of a school gardening intervention and children’s fruit and vegetable intake
This chapter will discuss the process evaluation undertaken in the two trials to explore the adherence to the different interventions (RHS-led and teacher-led) and identify how the different types of interventions implemented affected the primary outcome, children’s fruit and vegetable intake. This chapter captures gardening activity across all schools, including the control schools. With the nature of this type of intervention, schools will naturally tailor the intervention to their school’s needs. Therefore, they were pragmatic trials exploring whether or not the intervention worked in real-life conditions. By monitoring what activities are undertaken in school gardening, it is possible to explore whether or not the implementation level of the intervention was associated with dietary change in children’s fruit and vegetable intake.
Methodology
School gardening level interview
To identify the level of implementation and involvement of the schools in the RHS intervention, as well as identify if the control schools changed their level of involvement, the gardening telephone questionnaire was designed. The school gardening level is a measurement developed by the RHS to evaluate each school’s involvement in gardening, based on the following scale:56
-
level 0: no garden
-
level 1: planning
-
level 2: getting started
-
level 3: growing and diversifying
-
level 4: sharing best practice
-
level 5: celebrating with the wider community.
To move from one level to the next, the school needs to demonstrate more involvement in school gardening, in terms of development, teaching and interacting with the wider community. At baseline, each school completed a telephone interview to assess their gardening level. This interview was completed again at follow-up to assess change in gardening level. The questions from this questionnaire were based on the criteria that the RHS used to evaluate and assess schools (see Appendix 1).
Gardening process measures questionnaires
The main aim of the process evaluation was to capture details about the gardening activity within each school, identifying which fruit and vegetables each school grows and harvests. A gardening process measures questionnaire was designed to identify the different gardening activities occurring in each school and which year groups were involved. This information was captured via e-mail in September 2010 for both trials, and again at follow-up via e-mail in December 2011. The process measures questions are presented in Box 1. Both trials received the same e-mail.
Dear Schools,
Thank you so much for participating in the Evaluation of the RHS Campaign for School Gardening. We now have just seven questions we would like you to answer about gardening activities at your school that have occurred in the past year.
-
Do you have a school garden, if yes please describe (e.g. garden at the school, a few pots for growing plants in or an allotment)?
-
Which year groups are involved in gardening at your school?
-
Do you have a growing club or environmental club? If yes, which year groups are involved?
-
What fruit and vegetables has your school grown/tried to grow this summer?
-
What did you harvest?
-
What were your success/failure stories in the school garden this summer?
It is vital for the study that we collect information about your school garden, and if you need any help feel free to contact myself on the number below.
The information from this questionnaire was then collapsed into suitable variables to be used for analysis. Question 1 was broken into two variables. The variable of the question, ‘Do you have a school garden?’ was coded ‘yes’ or ‘no’. The second variable, gardening type, was coded:
-
0 = small: pots only
-
1 = medium: one to two raised beds
-
2 = large: more than two raised beds or school garden or an allotment near the school.
Question 2 was used in two different ways. The first concerned how many year groups are involved in the school garden, and was coded:
-
0 = key stage 1
-
1 = key stage 2
-
2 = all year groups.
The second was created to confirm if the year groups involved in the study were involved in school gardening. This question was coded ‘yes’ or ‘no’.
Question 3 was broken into two variables. Variable one, ‘Do you have a growing club or environmental club?’ was coded ‘yes’ or ‘no’. Variable two, gardening clubs, was coded into three groups using the same method as for question 2:
-
0 = key stage 1
-
1 = key stage 2
-
2 = all year groups.
Question 4 was broken down into two continuous variables:
-
frequency of different types of fruit grown – continuous
-
frequency of different types of vegetables grown – continuous.
The following variables were then created, as they consisted of the most commonly grown fruit and vegetables: tomatoes, lettuces, carrots, beans, corn, strawberries, apples and cucumbers.
Question 5, frequency of successfully harvested vegetables and fruit, was coded:
-
none
-
some
-
all fruit and vegetables grown were harvested.
Attendance of twilight sessions
The RHS regional advisor ran all of the twilight sessions; they were hosted at schools which received the RHS-led intervention, for the teacher-led intervention to attend. The RHS also provided Leeds University with information on the level of involvement in the twilight sessions of the teacher- and RHS-led schools.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analysis was performed using Stata version 12. Means and percentages for the process measures questions and general descriptive variables on the intervention implementation were generated.
School gardening level
The analysis was performed using clustered multilevel regression models with total fruit and vegetables as the primary outcome. The multilevel regression model was used to explore the difference in mean change in fruit and vegetable intake. These models were first conducted unadjusted, and then adjusted for gender, ethnicity and IMDS. The output generated for the primary analysis was mean, SE, 95% CI and p-value, with a p-value of < 0.05 taken to represent statistical significance for all of the analysis.
Results
Royal Horticultural Society-led school intervention gardening summary
The RHS-led schools all had major changes to their garden space over the course of the intervention. Table 45 presents a descriptive summary of these changes by region. This information was provided by the RHS regional advisor.
School | Baseline | Follow-up |
---|---|---|
Greenwich | ||
1 | There are two main areas: firstly, an allotment garden (derelict). This is a fairly large area currently set to grass at one end and also covered with landscape fabric and gravel. There are a few raised beds, and the last one-third of the area is blocked off by a solid wooden fence which is due to come down upon the completion of an adjacent building project. There is a large acer at both ends, with the one nearest the entrance providing shade for the grassed area. The second area is a sensory garden. This is in a courtyard area surrounded on three sides by high walls. It is in deep shade, and some thought should be taken as to planting | Now has raised beds (two groups of five RHS Wisley staff undertook team building days at the school and built 16 small raised beds and two large raised beds in a new garden area) |
2 | No specific ‘garden’ but there are planters/raised beds where growing is being carried out | Now has a fairly large school garden consisting of raised beds and a greenhouse. Bannockburn took part in the Hampton Court Flower Show’s scarecrow competition, celebrating characters from Lewis Carroll’s famous books Alice’s Adventures in Wonderland and Through the Looking Glass. Their ‘Mr Caterpillar’ gained a very respectable third prize in a field of more than 20 schools |
Tower Hamlets | ||
3 | The school currently has a wildlife and vegetable garden complete with pond. This area is due to be demolished to create new classrooms for this expanding school | A willow tunnel has been created. A new garden was being built over the 2011 summer holidays |
4 | Various ‘areas’: an excellent wildlife garden. A thriving raised pond, a spider’s web design wild flower meadow, plum tree, climbers, outdoor classroom. Key Stage 2 Years 3, 4, 5 and 6 have their own large planter in the playground. Key Stage 1 have four large planters. There are eight 1-m long beds. These are used by mums to grow stuff for the local co-op | Already had growing areas, but now have a shed and greenhouse yet to be erected. A Muslim Mums Group has taken part in two informal twilight sessions including seed sowing and pricking out seedlings |
5 | There is currently no gardening | Now has five raised beds for growing |
Sutton | ||
6 | A compact garden consisting of attractive gravel paths, four large raised beds and a fenced-off pond (including a small deck). There are other planters and beds around the school grounds, including some small planters in the Early Years playground planted up with herbs | In addition to their raised beds, now has a greenhouse |
7 | There is one main garden which has a number of beds and a thriving pond. A small newly cultivated bed in a shady area is planted up with a number of suitable plants. Due to fairly small total growing area, there is limited quantity, which affects the whole school exposure. There is currently no sheltered growing area to raise plants | Now has new beds built by parents (the school held two ‘Get Your Grown-ups Growing’ events over the winter, when parents took part in digging, and the construction of new beds) |
Wandsworth | ||
8 | The current garden is extremely impressive, but there is little provision for the children to grow (in terms of growing beds). The delightful garden is known as the ‘secret garden’ and has many features: a ‘human sundial’ in the centre, a small lawn, wildlife area with properly layered hedge, trees, a bog area, various benches and one small vegetable area (approximately 1.5 m × 3 m). There are also some raised brick planters in the main playground, which have mainly permanent planting and herbs | Now has two new growing areas. Development on the school grounds is ongoing |
9 | There are a number of areas set out for growing: main garden comprises 10 raised beds/planters (four of which are thin planters approximately 40 cm). A polycarbonate greenhouse has been purchased and is to be built next to the nursery garden. There are three raised beds in a separate courtyard area which Year 1 uses | In addition to its eight raised beds, the school now has two large growing beds built by parents (the school held a ‘Get Your Grown-ups Growing’ event over the winter) and a greenhouse |
10 | A few small raised beds in the main school garden which have been neglected somewhat. The timber is starting to break as the beds are made of a number of compost bin kits. There is no fence around the garden which allows the children to play on and in the beds. The school has acquired a large allotment plot (1-minute walk from the school). The aim is to turn this into a community garden, and use the produce for the school kitchens. This plot is totally overgrown at present | In addition to developing its own thriving school garden, the school has taken responsibility for a plot of land on the adjacent housing estate. This is to be a school community garden. In conjunction with the Residents Association and with the support from the RHS, this area is gradually being developed. This process has been assisted by a team of five gardeners from RHS Wisley who spent a day on the site building beds and on another day by a team of three gardeners who removed a large tree from the centre of the site. Additional raised beds were gifted to the garden by M&G Investments who sponsored a Chelsea show garden designed by Bunny Guinness. Twelve children from the school had the opportunity to visit the Flower Show to see the garden in situ and meet with Bunny Guinness. The RHS regional advisor assisted with all elements of development, including the co-ordination of removal of the M&G garden from Chelsea to Battersea |
Twilight sessions
For trial 1, all 10 of the RHS-led schools attended at least one twilight session, with a mean of 3.5 (SD 0.9) sessions attended. Of the schools which received the teacher-led intervention, only 4 out of 12 attended any of the twilight sessions, with a mean of 1.5 (SD 0.6) sessions attended. For trial 2, only two of the teacher-led schools attended any twilight sessions, with a mean of 1 (SD 0) session attended.
Implementation of gardening activities in schools in trial 1
For trial 1, at 6 months, four schools stated that they did not have a school garden (one from the RHS-led intervention group and three from the teacher-led intervention group). This was reduced to two schools in the teacher-led group and none in the RHS-led group by the end of the intervention period (Table 46). The number of vegetable types grown increased from 6 months to follow-up by an average of 1.3 for the RHS-led group, but there was no change in the number of fruits grown. In the teacher-led group, there was a decrease in number of types of fruits (0.9) and vegetables (1.7) grown from 6 months to follow-up. The number of schools that stated they had a large garden at 6 months was six for both groups; this increased to 7 out of 12 schools for the teacher-led group and 10 out of 10 schools for the RHS-led group at follow-up, showing an improvement in land allocated to school gardening. There was little change in the number of year groups involved in school gardening in either group, with eight schools in each having all year groups involved at follow-up. Schools were also asked to comment on the success of their fruit and vegetable harvest. These results show a decrease in success rate for the RHS-led schools, from four schools stating that they harvested all the fruit and vegetables they grew, to only two schools at follow-up. However, the teacher-led group had an increase from four schools to nine successfully harvesting all fruit and vegetables. This might explain in part why the teacher-led group had, on average, a higher change in combined fruit and vegetable intake compared with the RHS-led group.
Trial 1 process measures | 6 months | Follow-up | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Teacher-led, n (N = 12) | RHS-led, n (N = 10) | Teacher-led, n (N = 12) | RHS-led, n (N = 10) | |
Do you have a school garden? | ||||
No | 3 | 1 | 2 | 0 |
Yes | 9 | 9 | 10 | 10 |
Number of different fruits grown (mean; SD) | 9 (2.2; 1.9) | 8 (1.0; 1.1) | 10 (1.3; 1.7) | 10 (1.0; 1.2) |
Number of different vegetables grown (mean; SD) | 9 (7.0; 3.8) | 8 (6.0; 2.7) | 10 (5.3; 3.0) | 10 (7.3; 2.9) |
Size of garden | ||||
Small | 1 | 1 | 0 | 0 |
Medium | 2 | 2 | 2 | 0 |
Large | 6 | 6 | 7 | 10 |
Which year groups are involved? | ||||
Reception–year 2 | 0 | 1 | 0 | 1 |
Years 3–6 | 1 | 0 | 2 | 1 |
All | 7 | 8 | 8 | 8 |
Are years 3 and 4 involved (Yes) | 7 | 8 | 9 | 9 |
Do you have a gardening club? (Yes) | 6 | 6 | 6 | 7 |
Which year groups are involved in the gardening club? | ||||
Reception–year 2 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
Years 3–6 | 1 | 3 | 1 | 4 |
All | 3 | 3 | 3 | 2 |
Successfully harvested fruit and vegetables | ||||
None | 1 | 0 | 0 | 2 |
Some | 2 | 4 | 0 | 6 |
All | 4 | 4 | 9 | 2 |
Implementation of gardening activities in schools in trial 2
The results from the process measures e-mails for trial 2 are presented in Table 47. In the comparison group in trial 2, two schools were not involved in gardening at 6 months and this increased to three schools at follow-up. In the teacher-led group there was no change, with two schools stating that they did not have a school garden at 6 months and at follow-up. There was no change in the number of types of fruit grown in the comparison group and a marginal increase from 2.15 at 6 months to 2.33 at follow-up in the teacher-led group. There was more variation in number of vegetable types grown, with the mean in the comparison group increasing by 1.1 from 6 months to follow-up and the mean in the teacher-led group increasing by three. Again, there was little change in the comparison group in the number of schools that stated they had a large garden. However, this increased in the teacher-led group from 9 out of 15 schools at baseline to 12 at follow-up. Schools were also asked to comment on which year groups were involved in gardening. In three of the teacher-led schools, there was an increase from baseline to follow-up in the number of year groups involved in school gardening. Schools were also asked to comment on how successful their fruit and vegetable harvest was. These results show no increase in success rate for the comparison schools, with six schools on both occasions successfully harvesting all their fruit and vegetables. However, the teacher-led group had an increase from four to nine schools successfully harvesting all their fruit and vegetables.
Trial 2 process measures | 6 months | Follow-up | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Comparison, n (N = 15) | Teacher-led, n (N = 15) | Comparison, n (N = 15) | Teacher-led, n (N = 15) | |
Do you have a school garden? | ||||
No | 2 | 2 | 3 | 2 |
Yes | 13 | 13 | 12 | 13 |
Number of different fruits grown (mean; SD) | 13 (1.0; 1.6) | 13 (2.1; 2.6) | 12 (1.0; 1.3) | 12 (2.3; 2.1) |
Number of different vegetables grown (mean; SD) | 13 (4.6; 2.3) | 12 (7.0; 4.9) | 12 (5.7; 4.0) | 11 (10; 7.9) |
Size of garden | ||||
Small | 0 | 0 | 3 | 0 |
Medium | 5 | 2 | 1 | 1 |
Large | 8 | 9 | 7 | 12 |
Which year groups are involved? | ||||
Reception–year 2 | 0 | 1 | 2 | 1 |
Years 3–6 | 4 | 5 | 1 | 2 |
All | 9 | 7 | 8 | 10 |
Are years 3 and 4 involved? (Yes) | 11 | 11 | 9 | 11 |
Do you have a gardening club? (Yes) | 8 | 11 | 8 | 12 |
Which year groups are involved in the gardening club? | ||||
Reception–year 2 | 0 | 1 | 1 | 1 |
Years 3–6 | 4 | 5 | 4 | 6 |
All | 3 | 3 | 1 | 4 |
Successfully harvested fruit and vegetables | ||||
None | 1 | 0 | 2 | 0 |
Some | 4 | 5 | 3 | 4 |
All | 6 | 4 | 6 | 9 |
School gardening level
Table 48 displays the change in school gardening level for the RHS- and teacher-led interventions in trial 1. At baseline, 50% of the RHS-led schools only achieved a level 1 rating, compared with 60% of the schools at follow-up achieving level 3. This shows a large improvement in the quality of the garden, and gardening being integrated into the curriculum. The mean gardening level at follow-up was 2.7 for the RHS-led group compared with 1.9 for the teacher-led group. There was slightly more movement between the levels in the RHS-led group compared with the teacher-led group (a mean increase of 1.6 compared to 0.5). Multilevel regression analysis revealed that the difference between mean change in gardening level for the RHS-led compared with the teacher-led group was not significant (p = 0.06).
Gardening level | Baseline | Follow-up | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
RHS-led (N = 10) | Teacher-led (N = 12) | RHS-led (N = 10) | Teacher-led (N = 12) | |
Mean (SD) | 1.1 (0.7) | 1.4 (1.3) | 2.7 (1.1) | 1.9 (1.4) |
0, n (%) | 2 (20) | 2 (17) | 0 (0) | 2 (17) |
1, n (%) | 5 (50) | 7 (59) | 2 (20) | 3 (25) |
2, n (%) | 3 (30) | 1 (8) | 1 (10) | 3 (25) |
3, n (%) | 0 (0) | 1 (8) | 6 (60) | 3 (25) |
4, n (%) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) |
5, n (%) | 0 (0) | 1 (8) | 1 (10) | 1 (8) |
In trial 2 (Table 49), there was less movement between the gardening levels from baseline to follow-up. Although there was some change for both the teacher-led and control groups, multilevel regression analysis revealed that the difference between mean change in gardening level for the comparison compared with the teacher-led group was not significant (p = 0.7).
Gardening level | Baseline | Follow-up | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Control (N = 15) | Teacher-led (N = 15) | Control (N = 15) | Teacher-led (N = 15) | |
Mean (SD) | 1.3 (1.6) | 1 (1.2) | 1.8 (1.7) | 1.8 (1.2) |
0, n (%) | 6 (40) | 5 (32) | 3 (20) | 2 (14) |
1, n (%) | 5 (32) | 7 (47) | 6 (40) | 4 (25) |
2, n (%) | 2 (14) | 2 (14) | 1 (6) | 6 (40) |
3, n (%) | 2 (14) | 1 (7) | 2 (14) | 2 (14) |
4, n (%) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 1 (6) | 0 (0) |
5, n (%) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 2 (14) | 1 (7) |
Multilevel analysis
To explore whether or not change in gardening level from baseline to follow-up was associated with the primary outcome – combined fruit and vegetable intake – multilevel analysis was conducted using change in garden level score (follow-up minus baseline). These results are presented for trial 1 in Table 50 and for trial 2 in Table 51. The reference category in this model was no change, meaning that the schools did not change or improve in gardening level from baseline to follow-up. The effects on children’s fruit and vegetable intake after a change in one, two or three levels of gardening was compared with no change in gardening level. The results for all schools in trial 1 show that there was an increase in combined fruit and vegetable intake when schools improved by two levels or more. Increase by one level showed little or no change in children’s fruit and vegetable intake, while increasing by two levels when compared with no change improved children’s fruit and vegetable intake by 37 g, after adjusting for IMDS, ethnicity and gender. Change, however, was only significant when schools improved by three levels of the RHS gardening score; children from these schools increased their consumption of fruit and vegetables by 81 g, on average.
Change in gardening level | Number of schools | Number of pupils | Unadjusted | Adjusted for IMDS, age, ethnicity and gender | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean change in intake (g) | SE | p-value | Mean change in intake (g) | SE | 95% CI (g) | p-value | |||
No change (reference category) | 8 | 312 | 1 | 1 | |||||
Improved by 1 level | 4 | 132 | –4 | 26.3 | 0.8 | –5 | 26.9 | –58 to 46 | 0.8 |
Improved by 2 levels | 7 | 148 | 30 | 28.9 | 0.2 | 37 | 29.4 | –19 to 96 | 0.1 |
Improved by 3 levels | 2 | 49 | 68 | 41.8 | 0.1 | 81 | 42.0 | 0 to 163 | 0.05 |
Change in gardening level | Number of schools | Number of pupils | Unadjusted | Adjusted for IMDS, age, ethnicity and gender | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mean change in intake (g) | SE | p-value | Mean change in intake (g) | SE | 95% CI (g) | p-value | |||
No change (reference category) | 13 | 416 | 1 | 1 | |||||
Improved by 1 level | 11 | 360 | –24 | 33.6 | 0.4 | –30 | 34.3 | –98 to 36 | 0.3 |
Improved by 2 levels | 2 | 72 | –112 | 59.0 | 0.06 | –111 | 60.9 | –230 to 8 | 0.06 |
Improved by 3 levels | 3 | 65 | 55 | 58.6 | 0.3 | 44 | 61.1 | –74 to 164 | 0.4 |
However, this trend was not evident in trial 2. For change by one or two gardening levels there was a negative relationship between gardening level and children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Again, when schools improved by three gardening levels, children consumed on average 44 g more fruit and vegetables combined than children whose schools had no change in gardening level. However, these differences were not significant. Whereas trial 1 had a large proportion of schools improving by one or two gardening levels at follow-up compared with baseline, trial 2 had a large proportion of schools improving by one level, with only a few schools improving by two or three gardening ratings. This is to be expected, as in trial 1 all schools received an intervention, whereas in trial 2 some of the schools received no intervention.
Discussion
This chapter has explored the process evaluation undertaken in the two trials, to identify adherence to the different interventions (RHS-led and teacher-led) and how the different types of interventions implemented affected the primary outcome, children’s fruit and vegetable intake. The description of the 10 RHS-led intervention school gardens demonstrates a high level of involvement in the construction of gardening within these schools. This was observed in the change in school gardening level scores for these schools and the attendance rate for twilight sessions. In contrast, only 4 out of the 12 teacher-led schools in trial 1 attended any of the twilight sessions on offer to them, which might explain the lack of movement between the gardening levels. For trial 2, again, there was only a small amount of movement between the gardening levels for both the comparison and teacher-led groups. In both trials, there was no statistically significant difference between the intervention and control groups in terms of improvement in gardening level from baseline to follow-up. This is likely to have had an impact on the findings relating to fruit and vegetable intakes described in previous chapters.
Nevertheless, in all groups for both trials there were schools attempting to improve their gardening levels. In trial 1, 13 schools improved their gardening level and in trial 2, 16 schools improved their school gardening level by one level or more. This relationship with involvement in school gardening in trial 1 was associated with a significant change in children’s fruit and vegetable intake. In schools that improved by three levels, children on average consumed 81 g more fruit and vegetables than those in schools with no change in school gardening. In contrast, for trial 2, although there was an increase in fruit and vegetable consumption when schools improved by three levels in school gardening, this difference was not significant.
Theory behind school gardening
The main objectives for implementing gardening in schools are to improve educational knowledge of the environment, nutrition, and psychosocial and physical outcomes. 53,139–142 A lack of access to fruit and vegetables is considered one of the main barriers to consumption. Increasing children’s access to fruit and vegetables has been shown to increase consumption. 52 The school garden is considered an innovative way of teaching nutrition and health education; an alternative to classroom teaching that is hands-on and engages the children’s attention. 127 Although school gardening may be beneficial in educating children about fruit and vegetables and potentially increasing awareness and knowledge, the results from this study suggest that knowledge was already high, and to show any effect on children’s consumption levels, greater access and involvement in gardening may be needed.
Intervention design, elements and geographic location
Only five other studies have measured the relationship between children’s fruit and vegetable intake and a gardening intervention. 14,47,48,125,126 The interventions used in these studies ranged in length from 10 weeks to 2 years. Very little of the development of the gardens is described in these trials; however, the school garden was described as being 7.6 m2 in one study. 14
The fundamental aim of the RHS interventions was to introduce children to the basic gardening skills, such as planting, watering, weeding and harvesting. However, the five successful gardening interventions in these studies all involved additional elements in other settings as well as the gardening activities. Three interventions included cooking,47,48,125 two included nutrition education14,126 and one included parental newsletters and homework tasks. 48 Both the RHS-led and teacher-led interventions, however, were only implemented into additional curriculum lessons at the school’s desire. The primary focus of the RHS approach is to educate children in gardening. Including nutritional education or cooking alongside gardening might be required to achieve a positive change in children’s fruit and vegetable consumption. In one study, one of the additional classes for the students was ‘add a veggie to lunch day’. 14 These types of activities have shown positive results in improving children’s fruit and vegetable consumption. 127 It should also be noted that all of these successful gardening interventions have been implemented in countries with warmer climates than England – California, Minnesota, Alabama and Florida in the USA and Newcastle in Australia. Countries with sunnier summers may also be more successful because they can produce more for harvest, or because children can spend more time outside in the garden.
The interventions for this study were run by either the RHS regional advisor or teachers within each school. In some previously successful trials, teachers were used to implement the intervention. 47,48 If the classroom teacher is passionate about gardening, then this could assist with successful implementation of the intervention. 56 However, in other studies teachers not only taught the intervention but were also trained to complete the 24-hour food recall workbooks for the study. 14,126 Having the same people teach the intervention and collect the data could introduce bias into the results, as the teachers could have been motivated to demonstrate how well they have tried to implement the intervention. Only one study125 had an external company, the Youth Farmers and Market Project, similar to the RHS, implement their intervention and therefore reducing the risk of bias.
Barriers to implementing a school garden
School gardens require long-term commitment from the schools, and often need community assistance from parents if they are to be sustained. 54 Another issue found was that some schools took too long to establish the school garden, affecting the period of time in the studies for plants to germinate and grow edible fruit or vegetables. Environmental factors will also play an important role in the amount of food harvested. Schools are closed over summer, which is the peak harvesting season; without organising staff to water the garden and carry out general garden maintenance, the hard work during term time can be lost. With regard to the RHS school gardening levels, having grounds staff, caretakers or a school grounds maintenance contractors involved in the maintenance of the garden was only required for schools from level 3 onwards. The length of time spent in the interventions could also affect the chances of long-term change in children’s fruit and vegetable intake, with more sustained and intense intervention programmes more likely to have an impact on behaviour.
Limitations and strengths
There were limitations to the present study. The issues with the methodology of assessing dietary intake have been stated in previous chapters. Validity and reliability of the process measures questionnaires have not been tested; however, this is a common weakness with health interventions, as limited resources are allocated to process evaluations. For example, the question on harvesting success could have been interpreted in different ways by different teachers, with some perhaps interpreting success in terms of yield while others see it as involving all children in harvesting. Another limitation is that the study is subject to the well-established statistical problems of multiple comparisons or testing. This study was powered to analyse the main trial outcome, i.e. change in fruit and vegetable intake between children in the different treatment groups and, as a consequence, it may not be adequately powered for the process measures analysis. Furthermore, there was little apparent difference between groups in terms of gardening level improvement overall, potentially weakening the likelihood of detecting differences between groups as a result of gardening activity. It has to be recognised that these trials are being carried out in a ‘real life’ situation, so that those schools that were not receiving the RHS- or teacher-led interventions may have opted for other sources of gardening advice and activity. This is particularly likely to have occurred during the build-up to the 2012 Olympics year. Two of the eight London boroughs which formed the sampling frame (one from trial 1 and one from trial 2) straddle the main Olympic Park, where gardening was made a feature of non-athletic activity and a number of parks and gardens were created. In addition, the weather experienced in the winter of 2010 included the coldest December since records began. The summer of 2011 was also cooler and wetter than average.
The main strength of the present study is that it uses measures undertaken at different time points: baseline, 6 months and final follow-up. This has assisted in identifying change in gardening practices in not only the intervention schools, but also the comparison schools. Few studies explore in detail the implementation of the intervention.
Conclusion
The results from this chapter have demonstrated that, while there was no significant difference in the primary outcome of these trials, when gardening in schools is implemented at a high level it can have a positive effect on children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Previously successful gardening interventions indicate that future research needs to explore the involvement of additional activities to improve children’s consumption levels. This could be through the inclusion of nutritional education or cooking lessons. Parental involvement and parental consumption levels have always been considered pivotal, and should be incorporated into intervention designs.
When an intervention is run by teachers, it will naturally be tailored to meet their school’s needs. Nevertheless, the limitations to gardening interventions need to be acknowledged. Although gardening interventions might be able to assist in making small improvements in children’s knowledge of the environment, nutrition, and psychosocial and physical outcomes,53,139–142 additional intervention activities need to be integrated to produce lasting change in fruit and vegetable consumption.
Summary
In this chapter, the process evaluation undertaken in the two trials has been discussed. It has described the adherence to the different interventions (RHS-led and teacher-led) and has revealed that, for trial 1, if schools made substantial changes to their gardening level score from baseline to follow-up this could produce a positive effect on children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Nevertheless, in relation to intervention design, as discussed in this chapter, future research into school gardening should implement additional elements alongside gardening education, as the results from the current trials indicate that gardening on its own has very little impact on children’s fruit and vegetable intake.
Chapter 8 Summary discussion and recommendations for future research
Summary discussion
The interest in school gardening has grown over the past years, with some evidence that school gardening can provide children with a positive learning environment to help them improve their awareness and understanding of food and where it comes from, and possibly increase children’s willingness to consume fruit and vegetables. However, the evidence supporting these claims is based on research evaluating short-term interventions using small sample sizes. Despite the lack of funding, gardening in schools has increased in popularity, with gardening being added to the UK curricula for children in Key Stages 1–3 from September 2014. 143 The current two trials have found very little evidence to support the claims that school gardening can improve children’s fruit and vegetable intake (see Chapter 5). However, all groups had increased their gardening activity over the course of the study. The RHS-led group had increased the most, but there were no statistically significant differences between groups in gardening level at follow up. This lack of difference in gardening between groups may well have influenced the primary outcome. A high level of gardening, as characterised by the RHS levels, needs to be undertaken to produce a change in intake (see Chapter 7). The RHS considers that unless a head teacher is supportive of school gardening, despite their best efforts to improve children’s knowledge and attitudes, the positive efforts will produce little or no results. School and community gardens do provide other benefits even if they do not improve children’s fruit and vegetable intake, potentially improving psychological and social well-being in children. 139 Although these outcomes were not explored in the current study, it does demonstrate that, despite our findings relating to impact on diet, school gardens could be a useful educational tool.
In relation to improving children’s knowledge of and attitudes towards fruit and vegetables as a result of participating in a school gardening intervention, these two trials provide limited evidence to suggest that such an improvement takes place (see Chapter 6). For trial 1, the RHS-led gardening intervention was associated with an increase in the total number of types of vegetables recognised; however, this difference was not significant after adjustment for baseline measurement and possible confounders. A limitation of researching children’s knowledge of fruit and vegetables, or any other healthy nutrition education, is that there are very few validated tools. 144 More pilot research needs to be conducted to determine the reliability and validity of children’s knowledge questionnaires, one of the fundamental components of the SCT. 132
The process evaluations have provided some evidence to support previous research that school gardening can improve children’s fruit and vegetable intake (see Chapter 7). The results from this chapter have demonstrated that when gardening in schools is implemented at a high level, it can have a positive association with children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Previously successful gardening interventions suggest that future research needs to explore involving additional activities to improve children’s consumption levels. 47,126 This could be through including nutritional education or cooking lessons. Parental involvement and parents’ own consumption levels have always been considered pivotal, and should be incorporated into intervention designs. The RHS states that for a school garden to be successfully established, there are certain elements that are required. 145 The scheme must be supported in full by the head teacher. It is not suggesting that they need to be involved in the garden themselves; however, each school needs to identify how gardening will fit into the school day through including gardening in the school development plan. Examples of how this could be done would be ensuring that gardening is included across the curriculum, involving parents, identifying methods of linking in the community (such as through visiting a local allotment) and providing staff with the training necessary to be confident to teach gardening. Other examples are setting up a garden committee, as this will avoid pressure being placed on one teacher to maintain the garden, and helping to develop ongoing projects such as gardening clubs. Attempts need to be made to link in school gardening with the school catering company and/or staff, so that any produce grown can be included in school dinners to encourage children to taste what they have grown and be proud of their achievement. In addition, schools should attempt to use the produce from gardening in cooking lessons, to help children learn how to prepare the food themselves. 56
In addition to the RHS school gardening programme run in this study, the RHS is currently developing new resources for teachers to use in the classroom, with gardening-related themes such as ‘grow your own food for your lunchbox’. The fundamental principle behind these developments is to teach gardening in the curriculum to help children develop a lifelong love of gardening, growing and their environment. 56 It is should be noted that improving children’s fruit and vegetable intake is not one of the primary aims; nevertheless, the RHS hopes that educating children in gardening will in turn lead to an understanding of what they eat and where it comes from. Although gardening interventions may support small improvements in children’s knowledge of the environment, nutrition, and psychosocial and physical outcomes,53,139–142 additional intervention activities need to be integrated to produce lasting change in fruit and vegetable consumption.
Parents can help to facilitate change in their children’s fruit and vegetable intake. 77 Exploring the nutrient information collected at baseline has identified a positive public health message for parents, which could improve not only their own dietary habits, but also their children’s. This is the first large survey of London children to explore this association. It found that eating a family meal together at a table had the largest effect on children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Children in families who stated that they ate together every day had 1.5 more portions of fruit and vegetables daily than those whose families reported never eating together at a table, after adjusting for possible confounders. It also found that sometimes eating at a table together increased children’s fruit and vegetable consumption by more than a portion. The importance of the family meal is supported by previous research in preschool children99 and primary school children. 77,107,108 Parental intake is strongly associated with children’s intake,119,120 as was found in this study. Parents stating that they consumed fruit and vegetables more frequently was associated with higher consumption in their children.
This is the first study in the UK to identify that cutting up fruit and vegetables facilitates primary school-aged children’s intake. 77 If children have access to prepared fruit and vegetables at home, they are more likely to eat them. Research has been conducted in older children supporting this finding. 101,122 Future interventions could be tailored towards improving parental intake of fruit and vegetables, to facilitate children’s intake.
There are some barriers to implementing a school gardening programme. School gardens require long-term commitment if they are to be successfully established. 54 It is important to have a supportive team involved in the school garden to help maintain it over the summer months when the school is closed. The length of time spent in the interventions will also affect the chances of long-term change in children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Their consumption patterns are unlikely to be affected if their involvement in the actual intervention is limited.
The dietary assessment measurement used for these trials was a 24-hour recall tick list. The strength of the CADET diary is that it uses age- and gender-specific food portion sizes to calculate food and nutrient intake. The methodology used to administer the CADET diaries in schools was improved to help ensure completeness of the data collected. Children’s intake at school was recorded in CADET by trained fieldworkers and intake at home was recorded by parents/carers. An instructional DVD was sent home for parents to watch, to help them understand how to complete the CADET diary. Also, after the school food diary collection day, the fieldworker returned to the schools to collect and check the diaries with all the children, and if necessary conduct a retrospective recall. A 1-day tick list is an economically effective way of gathering nutrient information from children; however, it may not reflect true nutrient intake in the longer term. The CADET diary does avoid the issues with child self-reported food intake, and is less of a burden on the participants than the most commonly used alternative, a weighed 4-day food diary.
All analyses were conducted using multilevel analysis, a robust statistical methodology. The benefit of this technique is that the means and CIs for the different foods and nutrients will be more accurate; as children within a school are more similar to each other in terms of their food consumption, there will be less variability within the sample compared with a random sample from the whole population. 86,124 This level of analysis is rarely applied to the secondary outcomes, such as children’s knowledge and the process measures questionnaires.
Recommendations for future research
Despite the lack of evidence of a quantitative impact of school gardening on children’s intake, anecdotally, school gardening may have positive attributes. 54 When a school garden is successfully integrated into the school environment, it can provide a link between the community and the school. Beyond investigating school gardening initiatives, in order to increase children’s fruit and vegetable intakes, research needs to focus on intervention designs that tackle individual intake, family intake, school environment and the wider community. 54 The RHS believes that school gardening can provide vital links to members of the community who otherwise have little involvement with their child’s education,56 but this was not assessed in our study. This is supported in academic literature. 60,146 In order to fully appreciate how schools could be involved in improving children’s diets, a full review of the mechanisms of change and the major constraints, and the impact of both the external and wider school environments, such as school meals and food policies, is warranted. 39
Successful fruit and vegetable interventions in schools tended to have only a small impact on children’s fruit and vegetable intakes. 31 School gardening interventions that have identified a change in children’s diets have additional components. A recent systematic review of school-based interventions to improve children’s inactivity and nutrition knowledge stated that, for interventions to be successful, the vital components were integrating the intervention into the school curriculum, parental involvement through homework activities and developing a whole school approach through influencing changes to school policy around nutrition and physical activity education. 147,148 In Australia, a school gardening and cooking programme, the Stephanie Alexander Kitchen Garden Program, has had government support to develop the required infrastructure. 149 This programme has been funded by national and state support between 2008 and 2012, with the government spending $12.8M, approximately £8.7M, over 650 schools to develop cooking and gardening facilities. The Australian government has also recently invested an additional $5.4M (approximately £3.6M) on this programme. 149 The evaluation of this intervention has shown positive results for changing children’s behaviour in terms of fruit and vegetable intake, willingness to try fruit and vegetables and confidence in gardening and cooking skills. Future research into school gardening should be conducted with additional components such as cooking included, and parental involvement.
The WHO and the Food and Agriculture Organization believe that school-based interventions are a fundamental part of improving the population’s fruit and vegetable intake. 150 Approaches to increase support from industry and governments, to improve access to fruit and vegetables in all the settings in which children spend time, should be explored.
Future research should also be conducted to explore the effect of community gardens on children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Currently, there is a need for a robust study design to ascertain the role community gardens play as an intervention tool to improve children’s diets. Similar to school gardening, there are other benefits of community gardening besides focusing on fruit and vegetable consumption. Again, as with school gardening, community gardens are seen as a positive place for bringing different sections of the community together, and can have positive effects on the social well-being of the people involved. 151 Some community gardens have also been linked to school distribution programmes,52 while other studies have identified that community gardens can be used as a replacement for a school garden,60 with the community gardeners providing support and time to help local schools develop children’s knowledge of gardening. Schools involved with a community garden could elevate the responsibility of the school in running and maintaining the garden, which might make school gardening easier to maintain.
In addition to school-based intervention studies, there needs to be more focus on the home environment. We have identified the importance of eating together as a family to improve children’s fruit and vegetable intake. 77 Future intervention studies need to focus on parental involvement in supporting positive reinforcement and rewards around fruit and vegetable consumption, such as cutting up fruit and vegetables and eating fruit and vegetables together. A recent study stated that the barriers for parents are cost, family preferences and a limited choice of fruit and vegetables in restaurants. More pilot studies are needed that attempt to improve the home environment and to develop a suitable intervention to assist parents in overcoming these issues. 152
The quality of the tools used to evaluate these programmes is a further research concern. There are very few validated tools to explore nutrition knowledge in children; testing and developing these tools is essential to accurately measure children’s understanding of healthy dietary behaviour. Another limitation of measuring children’s knowledge is that, naturally, children do guess if they do not know the right answer. The design of nutrition knowledge questionnaires should always provide children with the option to write ‘don’t know’ – this could reduce the percentage of children guessing, and improve the questionnaire’s ability to accurately measure knowledge. Furthermore, knowledge questionnaires should be assessed for reliability. A possible method would be to use children’s school assessment scores and validity, through conducting a test–retest evaluation.
Conclusion
To conclude, this report has looked at the results from the first cluster RCTs designed to evaluate a school gardening intervention. The primary analysis from the two trials has found very little evidence to support the claims that school gardening alone can improve children’s fruit and vegetable intake. In both trials, the gardening levels increased across all groups and there was no statistically significant difference between the intervention and control groups in terms of improvement in gardening level from baseline to follow-up. This lack of differentiation between groups is likely to have influenced the primary outcome. However, process measures evaluation found that when the gardening intervention was implemented at the highest intensities within the schools, there was a suggestion that it could improve children’s fruit and vegetable intake by a portion. Improving children’s fruit and vegetable intake remains a challenging task. This study highlights the need for more sophisticated and accurate tools to evaluate diet in children. Future intervention designs should include a greater level of parental involvement in school interventions, along with related components such as cooking, to substantially improve children’s fruit and vegetable intake. In addition, the home environment has been demonstrated to be an important focus for intervention.
Acknowledgements
We acknowledge the contribution of Neil Hancock as project database manager; Claire McLoughlin who provided administrative support; Dr Jayne Hutchinson who undertook the analysis presented in Chapter 6 on attitudes and knowledge; Camilla Nykjaer who supported the data collection; Jim Bliss and Deidre Walton from the RHS Campaign for School Gardening; and Emily Cade, who was the voice of Tommy Tomato. We appreciate the time given by the trial steering committee to support the project: Dr Cindy Cooper, Graeme Slate, Deidre Walton and Maria Bryant. Thanks to all the nutrition students who were trained as fieldworkers and worked with the schools to collect the data.
We thank all the schools and children who gave up their time to take part in this study.
Contributions of authors
Meaghan S Christian (research student, Nutritional Epidemiology Group) was responsible for development and organisation of the trials. She trained fieldworkers and undertook data collection. She developed and piloted the questionnaires, adapting CADET for use in this slightly older age group. She analysed the baseline data and the full trial analysis as well as the process measures analysis. She provided the majority of the text for this report and also wrote a PhD thesis as a result of this project ‘Can a school gardening programme lead to improved fruit and vegetable intake in children?’ PhD 2014, University of Leeds.
Dr Charlotte EL Evans (Lecturer in Nutrition, Nutritional Epidemiology Group) was responsible for overseeing the statistical analysis as the trial statistician. She supervised Meaghan Christian during the project.
Professor Janet E Cade (Professor of Nutritional Epidemiology and Public Health) conceived the idea of the project. She provided day-to-day oversight of the project activity. She supervised Meaghan Christian and provided input to the development of the methods and analysis.
Disclaimers
This report presents independent research funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR). The views and opinions expressed by authors in this publication are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect those of the NHS, the NIHR, NETSCC, the PHR programme or the Department of Health. If there are verbatim quotations included in this publication the views and opinions expressed by the interviewees are those of the interviewees and do not necessarily reflect those of the authors, those of the NHS, the NIHR, NETSCC, the PHR programme or the Department of Health.
References
- Global Strategy on Diet, Physical Activity and Health. Geneva: WHO; 2004.
- Butland B, Jebb S, Kopelman P, McPherson K, Thomas S, Mardell J, et al. Foresight Tackling Obesities: Future Choices – Project Report. London: Foresight; 2012.
- Food, Nutrition, Physical Activity, and the Prevention of Cancer: a Global Perspective. Washington, DC: American Institute for Cancer Research; 2007.
- Obesity, An Epidemic. Report of the Meeting. Geneva: WHO; 2009.
- Horton R. GBD 2010: understanding disease, injury, and risk. Lancet 2012;380:2053-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62133-3.
- Singer MR, Moore LL, Garahie EJ, Ellison RC. The tracking of nutrient intake in young children: the Framingham children’s study. Am J Public Health 1995;85:1673-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.85.12.1673.
- Skinner JD, Carruth BR, Wendy N, Ziegler PJ. Children’s food preferences: a longitudinal analysis. J Am Diet Assoc 2002;102:1638-47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0002-8223(02)90349-4.
- Fisher JO, Mitchell DC, Smiciklas-Wright H, Birch LL. Parental influences on young girls’ fruit and vegetable, micronutrient, and fat intakes. J Am Diet Assoc 2002;102:58-64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0002-8223(02)90017-9.
- Statistics on Obesity, Physical Activity and Diet: England, 2012. Leeds: Health and Social Care Information Centre; 2012.
- Obesity Among Children Under 11. London: Department of Health; 2005.
- Nicklas T, Johnson C, Farris R, Rice R, Lyon L, Shi R. Development of a school-based nutrition intervention for high school students: Gimme 5. Am J Health Promot 1997;11:315-22. http://dx.doi.org/10.4278/0890-1171-11.5.315.
- Maynard M, Gunnell D, Emmett P, Frankel S, Davey Smith G. Fruit, vegetables, and antioxidants in childhood and risk of adult cancer: the Boyd Orr cohort. J Epidemiol Community Health 2003;57:218-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.57.3.218.
- Perez Rodrigo C, Aranceta J, Brug H, Wind M, Hildonen C, Klepp KI, et al. School-based education strategies to promote fruit and vegetable consumption: the Pro Children Project. Arch Latinoam Nutr 2004;54:14-9.
- McAleese JD, Rankin LL. Garden-based nutrition education affects fruit and vegetable consumption in sixth-grade adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc 2007;107:662-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2007.01.015.
- Summerbell CD, Ashton V, Campbell KJ, Edmunds L, Kelly S, Waters E, et al. Interventions for treating obesity in children. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2009;1.
- Magarey A, Daniels LA, Smith A. Fruit and vegetable intakes of Australians aged 2–18 years: an evaluation of the 1995 National Nutrition Survey data. Aust N Z J Public Health 2001;25:155-61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1753-6405.2001.tb01839.x.
- Timperio A, Ball K, Roberts R, Campbell K, Andrianopoulos N, Crawford D, et al. Children’s fruit and vegetable intake: associations with the neighbourhood food environment. Prev Med 2008;46:331-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2007.11.011.
- National Diet and Nutrition Survey: Headline Results from Years 1 and 2 (Combined) of the Rolling Programme 2008/9–2009/10. London: Department of Health; 2011.
- Nelson M, Erens B, Bates B, Church S, Boshier T. Low Income Diet and Nutrition Survey. London: The Stationery Office; 2007.
- Connolly J, Gargiula L, Reeve D. Selections from current literature. Treatment issues in childhood obesity. Fam Pract 2002;19:304-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/fampra/19.3.304.
- Cullen KW, Baranowski T, Baranowski J, Warnecke C, de Moor C, Nwachokor A, et al. ‘5 A Day’ achievement badge for urban boy scouts: formative evaluation results. J Cancer Educ 1998;13:162-8.
- Ciliska D, Miles E, Brien MA, Turl C, Tomasik HH, Donovan U. The Effectiveness of Community Interventions to Increase Fruit and Vegetable Consumption in People Four Years of Age and Older. Ontario: Ontario Ministry of Health Department; 1999.
- Baranowski J, Doyle C, Lin LS, Smith M, Wang DT. Gimme 5 fruit, juice, and vegetables for fun and health: outcome evaluation. Health Educ Behav 2000;27:96-111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109019810002700109.
- Patrick K, Sallis JF, Prochaska JJ, Lydston DD, Calfas KJ, Zabinski MF, et al. A multicomponent program for nutrition and physical activity change in primary care: PACE+ for adolescents. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2001;155:940-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.155.8.940.
- Bere E, Veierod MB, Klepp K-I. The Norwegian School Fruit Programme: evaluating paid vs. no-cost subscriptions. Prev Med 2005;41:463-70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2004.11.024.
- Wells L, Nelson M. The National School Fruit Scheme produces short-term but not longer-term increases in fruit consumption in primary school children. Br J Nutr 2005;93:537-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1079/BJN20051393.
- Yeo ST, Edwards RT. Encouraging fruit consumption in primary schoolchildren: a pilot study in North Wales, UK. J Hum Nutr Diet 2006;19:299-302. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-277X.2006.00706.x.
- Fogarty AW, Antoniak M, Venn AJ, Davies L, Goodwin A, Salfield N, et al. Does participation in a population-based dietary intervention scheme have a lasting impact on fruit intake in young children?. Intl J Epidemiol 2007;36:1080-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ije/dym133.
- Perry CL, Bishop DB, Taylor G, Murray DM, Mays RW, Dudovitz BS. Changing fruit and vegetable consumption among children: the 5-a-Day Power Plus program in St. Paul, Minnesota. Am J Public Health 1998;88:603-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.88.4.603.
- Reynolds KD, Franklin FA, Binkley D, Raczynski JM, Harrington KF, Kirk KA, et al. Increasing the fruit and vegetable consumption of fourth-graders: results from the high 5 project. Prev Med 2000;30:309-19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/pmed.1999.0630.
- Evans CE, Christian MS, Cleghorn CL, Greenwood DC, Cade JE. Systematic review and meta-analysis of school-based interventions to improve daily fruit and vegetable intake in children aged 5 to 12 y. Am J Clin Nutr 2012;96:889-901. http://dx.doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.111.030270.
- Cullen KW, Watson K, Baranowski T, Baranowski JH, Zakeri I, Cullen KW, et al. Squire’s Quest: intervention changes occurred at lunch and snack meals. Appetite 2005;45:148-51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2005.04.001.
- Bere E, Veierod MB, Bjelland M, Klepp KI. Outcome and process evaluation of a Norwegian school-randomized fruit and vegetable intervention: Fruits and Vegetables Make the Marks (FVMM). Health Educ Res 2006;21:258-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/her/cyh062.
- Te Velde SJ, Brug J, Wind M, Hildonen C, Bjelland M, Perez-Rodrigo C, et al. Effects of a comprehensive fruit- and vegetable-promoting school-based intervention in three European countries: the Pro Children Study. Br J Nutr 2008;99:893-90. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S000711450782513X.
- Auld GW, Romaniello C, Heimendinger J, Hambidge C, Hambidge M. Outcomes from a school-based nutrition education program using resource teachers and cross-disciplinary models. J Nutr Educ 1998;30:268-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3182(98)70336-X.
- Sahota P, Rudolf MC, Dixey R, Hill AJ, Barth JH, Cade J, et al. Randomised controlled trial of primary school based intervention to reduce risk factors for obesity. BMJ 2001;323:1029-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.323.7320.1029.
- Perry CL, Bishop DB, Taylor GL, Davis M, Story M, Gray C, et al. A randomized school trial of environmental strategies to encourage fruit and vegetable consumption among children. Health Educ Behav 2004;31:65-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1090198103255530.
- French SA, Wechsler H. School-based research and initiatives: fruit and vegetable environment, policy, and pricing workshop. Prev Med 2004;39:101-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2003.10.007.
- Howerton MW, Bell BS, Dodd KW, Berrigan D, Stolzenberg-Solomon R, Nebeling L, et al. School-based nutrition programs produced a moderate increase in fruit and vegetable consumption: meta and pooling analyses from 7 studies. J Nutr Educ Behav 2007;39:186-96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2007.01.010.
- Bandura A. Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective. Annu Rev Psychol 2001;52:1-26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1.
- Poston S, Shoemaker C, Dzewaltowski D. A comparison of a gardening and nutrition program with a standard nutrition program in an out-of-school setting. HortTechnology 2005;15:463-7.
- Koch S, Waliczek T, Zajicek J. The effect of a summer garden program on the nutritional knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors of children. HortTechnology 2006;16:620-4.
- O’Brien S, Shoemaker C. An after-school gardening club to promote fruit and vegetable consumption among fourth grade students: The assessment of the social cognitive theory constructs. HortTechnology 2006;16:24-9.
- Morris J, Neustadter A, Zidenberg-Cherr S. First-grade gardeners more likely to taste vegetables. Calif Agric 2001;55:43-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.3733/ca.v055n01p43.
- Cason KL. Children are ‘growing healthy’ in South Carolina. J Nutr Educ 1999;31:235-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3182(99)70446-2.
- Morris JL, Zidenberg-Cherr S. Garden-enhanced nutrition – Education improves vegetable preferences of fourth-grade school children. Faseb J 2002;14.
- Morgan PJ, Warren JM, Lubans DR, Saunders KL, Quick GI, Collins CE. The impact of nutrition education with and without a school garden on knowledge, vegetable intake and preferences and quality of school life among primary-school students. Public Health Nutr 2010;13:1931-40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980010000959.
- Wang MC, Rauzon S, Studer N, Martin AC, Craig L, Merlo C, et al. Exposure to a comprehensive school intervention increases vegetable consumption. J Adolesc Health 2010;47:74-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.12.014.
- O’Dea J. Why do kids eat healthful food? Perceived benefits of and barriers to healthful eating and physical activity among children and adolescents. J Am Diet Assoc 2003;103:497-501.
- Evans CEL. A Cluster Randomised Controlled Trial to Improve Primary School Children’s Packed Lunches in the UK 2010.
- Hatloy A, Torheim L, Oshaug A. Food variety – a good indicator of nutritional adequacy of the diet? A case study from an urban area in Mali, West Africa. Eur J Clin Nutr 1997;52:891-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1600662.
- Blair D. The child in the garden: an evaluative review of the benefits of school gardening. J Environ Educ 2009;40:15-38. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/JOEE.40.2.15-38.
- Somerset S, Ball R, Flett M, Rebecca G. School-based community gardens: re-establishing healthy relationships with food. J HEIA 2005;12:25-33.
- Ozer EJ. The effects of school gardens on students and schools: conceptualization and considerations for maximizing healthy development. Health Educ Behav 2007;34:846-63. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1090198106289002.
- Robinson-O’Brien R, Story M, Heim S. Impact of garden-based youth nutrition intervention programs: a review. J Am Diet Assoc 2009;109:273-80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2008.10.051.
- Royal Horticultural Society . Gardening in Schools. A Vital Tool For Children’s Learning n.d. http://apps.rhs.org.uk/schoolgardening/teachershome/news/researchonschoolgardening.aspa (accessed 8 May 2014).
- Krolner R, Suldrup Jorgensen T, Aarestrup AK, Hjollund Christiansen A, Christensen AM, Due P. The Boost study: design of a school- and community-based randomised trial to promote fruit and vegetable consumption among teenagers. BMC Public Health 2012;12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-12-191.
- De Bourdeaudhuij I, te Velde S, Brug J, Due P, Wind M, Sandvik C, et al. Personal, social and environmental predictors of daily fruit and vegetable intake in 11-year-old children in nine European countries. Eur J Clin Nutr 2008;62:834-41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602794.
- Patrick H, Nicklas TA. A review of family and social determinants of children’s eating patterns and diet quality. J Am Coll Nutr 2005;24:83-92. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07315724.2005.10719448.
- Somerset S. The potential for school gardens to enhance health. International Fruit and Vegetable Alliance Scientific Newsletter 2009;35.
- Resnicow K, Davis M, Smith M, Lazarus-Yaroch A, Baranowski T, Baranowski J, et al. How best to measure implementation of school health curricula: a comparison of three measures. Health Educ Res 1998;13:239-50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/her/13.2.239.
- Cade JE, Frear L, Greenwood DC. Assessment of diet in young children with an emphasis on fruit and vegetable intake: using CADET – Child and Diet Evaluation Tool. Public Health Nutr 2006;9:501-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1079/PHN2005871.
- Ransley JK, Greenwood DC, Cade JE, Blenkinsop S, Schagen I, Teeman D, et al. Does the school fruit and vegetable scheme improve children’s diet? A non-randomised controlled trial. J Epidemiol Community Health 2007;61:699-703. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.2006.052696.
- Ransley JK, Taylor EF, Radwan Y, Kitchen MS, Greenwood DC, Cade JE. Does nutrition education in primary schools make a difference to children’s fruit and vegetable consumption?. Public Health Nutr 2010;13:1898-904. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980010000595.
- Kitchen MS, Ransley JK, Greenwood DC, Clarke GP, Conner MT, Jupp JE. Study protocol: a cluster randomised controlled trial of a school based fruit and vegetable intervention – Project Tomato. BMC Health Serv Res 2009;9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1472-6963-9-101.
- Evans CE, Ransley JK, Christian MS, Greenwood DC, Thomas JD, Cade JE. A cluster-randomised controlled trial of a school-based fruit and vegetable intervention: Project Tomato. Public Health Nutr 2013;16:1073-81. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980012005290.
- Holland B, Welch AA, Unwin ID, Buss DH, Paul AA, Southgate DAT. McCance and Widdowson’s The Composition of Foods. Cambridge: The Royal Society of Chemistry; 1991.
- Gregory J, Lowe S, Bates CJ, Prentice A, Jackson L, Smithers G. National Diet and Nutrition Survey: Young People Aged 4 to 18 Years. London: The Stationery Office; 2001.
- Kristjansdottir AG, Thorsdottir I, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Due P, Wind M, Klepp K-I. Determinants of fruit and vegetable intake among 11-year-old schoolchildren in a country of traditionally low fruit and vegetable consumption. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity 2006;3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-3-41.
- De Bourdeaudhuij I, Klepp K, Due P, Perez Rodrigo C, de Almeida M, Wind M, et al. Reliability and validity of a questionnaire to measure personal, social and environmental correlates of fruit and vegetable intake in 10–11-year-old children in five European countries. Public Health Nutr 2005;8:189-200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1079/PHN2004673.
- Bryant M, Stevens J. Measurement of food availability in the home. Nutr Rev 2006;64:67-76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1753-4887.2006.tb00189.x.
- Bryant M, Ward D, Hales D, Vaughn A, Tabak R, Stevens J. Reliability and validity of the Healthy Home Survey: A tool to measure factors within homes hypothesized to relate to overweight in children. IJBNPA 2008;5. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-5-23.
- Somerset S, Markwell K. Impact of a school-based food garden on attitudes and identification skills regarding vegetables and fruit: a 12-month intervention trial. Public Health Nutr 2009;12:214-21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980008003327.
- Molag ML, de Vries JH, Ocké MC, Dagnelie PC, van den Brandt PA, Jansen MCJF, et al. Design characteristics of food frequency questionnaires in relation to their validity. Am J Epidemiol 2007;166:1468-78. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwm236.
- Livingstone MBE, Robson PJ. Measurement of dietary intake in children. Proc Nutr Soc 2000;59:279-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0029665100000318.
- Cade J, Thompson R, Burley V, Warm D. Development, validation and utilisation of food-frequency questionnaires – a review. Public Health Nutr 2002;5:567-87. http://dx.doi.org/10.1079/PHN2001318.
- Christian MS, Evans CE, Ransley JK, Greenwood DC, Thomas JD, Cade JE. Process evaluation of a cluster randomised controlled trial of a school-based fruit and vegetable intervention: Project Tomato. Public Health Nutr 2012;15:459-65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980011001844.
- Hughes RJ, Edwards KL, Clarke GP, Evans CE, Cade JE, Ransley JK. Childhood consumption of fruit and vegetables across England: a study of 2306 6–7-year-olds in 2007. Br J Nutr 2012;108:733-42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0007114511005939.
- Gibney MJ, Margetts BM, Kearney JM, Arab L. Public Health Nutrition. Oxford: Blackwell Science; 2006.
- Lillegaard IT, Loken EB, Andersen LF. Relative validation of a pre-coded food diary among children, under-reporting varies with reporting day and time of the day. Eur J Clin Nutr 2007;61:61-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602487.
- Christian MS, Evans CEL, Conner M, Ransley JK, Cade JE. Study protocol: can a school gardening intervention improve children’s diets?. BMC Public Health 2012;12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-12-304.
- Campbell MK, Elbourne DR, Altman DG. CONSORT group . CONSORT statement: extension to cluster randomised trials. BMJ 2004;328:702-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.328.7441.702.
- Schulz KF, Altman DG, Moher D. CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. J Clin Epidemiol 2010;63:834-40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.02.005.
- Juul S, Frydenberg M. An Introduction to Stata for Health Researchers. College Station, TX: Stata Press; 2010.
- Snijders T, Bosker R. 2nd Edition Multilevel Analysis: An Introduction to Basic and Advanced Multilevel Modeling. London: SAGE Publications; 2012.
- Rasbash J, Steele F, Browne WJ, Prosser B. A User’s Guide to MLwiN, Version 2.0. London: Institute of Education; 2004.
- Pevalin D, Robson K. The Stata Survival Manual. Maidenhead, Berkshire: Open University Press; 2011.
- Energy and Protein Requirements: Report of a Joint Expert Consultation. Geneva: WHO; 1985.
- 41 Dietary Reference Values for Food Energy and Nutrients for the United Kingdom. London: HMSO; 1991.
- Nutrient Reference Values (NRVs) for Australia and New Zealand Including Recommended Dietary Intake. Canberra: NHMRC; 2006.
- Ness AR, Powles JW. Fruit and vegetables, and cardiovascular disease: a review. Int J Epidemiol 1997;26:1-13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ije/26.1.1.
- Bazzano LA, He J, Ogden LG, Loria CM, Vupputuri S, Myers L, et al. Fruit and vegetable intake and risk of cardiovascular disease in US adults: the first National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey epidemiologic follow-up study. Am J Clin Nutr 2002;76:93-9.
- Boffetta P, Couto E, Wichmann J, Ferrari P, Trichopoulos D, Bueno-de-Mesquita HB, et al. Fruit and vegetable intake and overall cancer risk in the European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition (EPIC). J Natl Cancer Inst 2010;102:529-37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jnci/djq072.
- Harding AH, Wareham NJ, Bingham SA, Khaw K, Luben R, Welch A, et al. Plasma vitamin C level, fruit and vegetable consumption, and the risk of new-onset type 2 diabetes mellitus: the European prospective investigation of cancer – Norfolk prospective study. Arch Intern Med 2008;168:1493-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archinte.168.14.1493.
- Reilly JJ, Kelly L, Montgomery C, Williamson A, Fisher A, McColl JH, et al. Physical activity to prevent obesity in young children: cluster randomised controlled trial. BMJ 2006;333:1041-3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.38979.623773.55.
- NHS . Change4Life 2012. www.nhs.uk/Change4Life/Pages/change-for-life.aspx (accessed 8 May 2014).
- Cooke L. The importance of exposure for healthy eating in childhood: a review. J Hum Nutr Diet 2007;20:294-301. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-277X.2007.00804.x.
- Jones LR, Steer CD, Rogers IS, Emmett PM. Influences on child fruit and vegetable intake: sociodemographic, parental and child factors in a longitudinal cohort study. Public Health Nutr 2010;13:1122-30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980010000133.
- Gibson EL, Wardle J, Watts CJ. Fruit and vegetable consumption, nutritional knowledge and beliefs in mothers and children. Appetite 1998;31:205-28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/appe.1998.0180.
- French SA, Stables G. Environmental interventions to promote vegetable and fruit consumption among youth in school settings. Prev Med 2003;37:593-610. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2003.09.007.
- Cullen WK, Baranowski T, Rittenberry L, Cosart C, Owens E, Hebert D, et al. Socioenvironmental influences on children’s fruit, juice and vegetable consumption as reported by parents: reliability and validity of measures. Public Health Nutr 2000;3:345-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980000000392.
- Reilly JJ, Dorosty AR, Emmett PM. Prevalence of overweight and obesity in British children: cohort study. BMJ 1999;319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.319.7216.1039.
- Miller RG. Simultaneous Statistical Inference. New York, NY: Springer Verlag; 1981.
- Salt and Health. London: The Stationery Office; 2003.
- Glynn L, Emmett P, Rogers I, Team AS. Food and nutrient intakes of a population sample of 7-year-old children in the south-west of England in 1999/2000 – what difference does gender make?. J Hum Nutr Diet 2005;18:7-19. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-277X.2004.00582.x.
- Inchley J, Todd J, Bryce C, Currie C. Dietary trends among Scottish schoolchildren in the 1990s. J Hum Nutr Diet 2001;14:207-16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-277X.2001.00285.x.
- Cullen KW, Baranowski T, Rittenberry L, Cosart C, Hebert D, de Moor C. Child-reported family and peer influences on fruit, juice and vegetable consumption: reliability and validity of measures. Health Educ Res 2001;16:187-200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/her/16.2.187.
- Rockett HRH. Family dinner: more than just a meal. J Am Diet Assoc 2007;107:1498-501. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2007.07.004.
- Gribble LS, Falciglia G, Davis AM, Couch SC. A curriculum based on social learning theory emphasizing fruit exposure and positive parent child-feeding strategies: a pilot study. J Am Diet Assoc 2003;103:100-3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1053/jada.2003.50011.
- Fitzpatrick E, Edmunds LS, Dennison BA. Positive effects of family dinner are undone by television viewing. J Am Diet Assoc 2007;107:666-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2007.01.014.
- Fulkerson JA, Story M, Neumark-Sztainer D, Rydell S. Family meals: perceptions of benefits and challenges among parents of 8- to 10-year-old children. J Am Diet Assoc 2008;108:706-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2008.01.005.
- Robinson-O’Brien R, Neumark-Sztainer D, Hannan PJ, Burgess-Champoux T, Haines J. Fruits and vegetables at home: child and parent perceptions. J Nutr Educ Behav 2009;41:360-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2008.08.003.
- McIntosh WA, Kubena KS, Tolle G, Dean WR, Jan JS, Anding J, et al. Mothers and meals. The effects of mothers’ meal planning and shopping motivations on children’s participation in family meals. Appetite 2010;55:623-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.09.016.
- Matheson DM, Killen JD, Wang Y, Varady A, Robinson TN. Children’s food consumption during television viewing. Am J Clin Nutr 2004;79:1088-94.
- Kelder SH, Perry CL, Klepp KI, Lytle LL. Longitudinal tracking of adolescent smoking, physical activity, and food choice behaviors. Am J Public Health 1994;84:1121-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.84.7.1121.
- Kratt P, Reynolds K, Shewchuk R. The role of availability as a moderator of family fruit and vegetable consumption. Health Educ Behav 2000;27:471-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/109019810002700409.
- Taveras EM, Rifas-Shiman SL, Berkey CS, Rockett HRH, Field AE, Frazier AL, et al. Family dinner and adolescent overweight. Obesity 2005;13:900-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.2005.104.
- NHS Future Forum . The NHS’s Role in the Public’s Health n.d. www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/216423/dh_132114.pdf (accessed 25 April 2014).
- Blanchette L, Brug J. Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among 6–12-year-old children and effective interventions to increase consumption. J Hum Nutr Diet 2005;18:431-43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-277X.2005.00648.x.
- Wind M, Bjelland M, Perez-Rodrigo C, Te Velde SJ, Hildonen C, Bere E, et al. Appreciation and implementation of a school-based intervention are associated with changes in fruit and vegetable intake in 10- to 13-year old schoolchildren – the Pro Children study. Health Educ Res 2008;23:997-1007. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/her/cym078.
- Jago R, Baranowski T, Baranowski JC. Fruit and vegetable availability: a micro environmental mediating variable?. Public Health Nutr 2007;10:681-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1368980007441441.
- Wind M, de Bourdeaudhuij I, Te Velde SJ, Sandvik C, Due P, Klepp KI, et al. Correlates of fruit and vegetable consumption among 11-year-old Belgian-Flemish and Dutch schoolchildren. J Nutr Educ Behav 2006;38:211-21. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2006.02.011.
- The Mayor’s Educational Inquiry First Report. Greater London Authority; 2012.
- Aiken L, West S. Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1991.
- Lautenschlager L, Smith C. Understanding gardening and dietary habits among youth garden program participants using the Theory of Planned Behavior. Appetite 2007;49:122-30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2007.01.002.
- Parmer SM, Salisbury-Glennon J, Shannon D, Struempler B. School gardens: an experiential learning approach for a nutrition education program to increase fruit and vegetable knowledge, preference, and consumption among second-grade students. J Nutr Educ Behav 2009;41:212-17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2008.06.002.
- Knai C, Pomerleau J, Lock K, McKee M. Getting children to eat more fruit and vegetables: a systematic review. Prev Med 2006;42:85-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2005.11.012.
- Hermann JR, Parker SP, Brown BJ, Siewe YJ, Denney BA, Walker SJ. After-school gardening improves children’s reported vegetable intake and physical activity. J Nutr Educ Behav 2006;38:201-2. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2006.02.002.
- Heim S, Bauer KW, Stang J, Ireland M. Can a community-based intervention improve the home food environment? Parental perspectives of the influence of the delicious and nutritious garden. J Nutr Educ Behav 2011;43:130-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2010.01.003.
- Vickers AJ, Altman DG. Statistics notes: Analysing controlled trials with baseline and follow up measurements. BMJ 2001;323:1123-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.323.7321.1123.
- Bandura A. Social Foundations of Thought and Action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1986.
- Morris JL, Briggs M, Zideberg-Cherr S. School-based gardens can teach kids healthier eating habits. Calif Agric 2000;54:40-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.3733/ca.v054n05p40.
- Ratcliffe MM, Merrigan KA, Rogers BL, Goldberg JP. The effects of school garden experiences on middle school-aged students’ knowledge, attitudes, and behaviors associated with vegetable consumption. Health Promot Pract 2011;12:36-43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1524839909349182.
- Rasmussen M, Krolner R, Klepp K-I, Lytle L, Brug J, Bere E, et al. Determinants of fruit and vegetable consumption among children and adolescents: a review of the literature. Part I: quantitative studies. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity 2006;3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-3-22.
- Genzer SR, Seagraves L, Whittlesey CW, Robinson S, Koch EA. Junior Master Gardener Level 1 Golden Ray – Health and Nutrition From the Garden. Bryan, TX: Newman Publishing; 2001.
- Lineberger S, Zajicek J. School gardens: Can a hands-on teaching tool affect students’ attitudes and behaviors regarding fruit and vegetables?. HortTechnology 2000;10:593-7.
- Davis JN, Ventura EE, Cook LT, Gyllenhammer LE, Gatto NM. Sprouts: a gardening, nutrition, and cooking intervention for Latino youth improves diet and reduces obesity. J Am Diet Assoc 2011;111:1224-30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2011.05.009.
- Newell SA, Huddy AD, Adams JK, Miller M, Holden L, Dietrich UC. The tooty fruity vegie project: changing knowledge and attitudes about fruits and vegetables. Aust N Z J Public Health 2004;28:288-95. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-842X.2004.tb00489.x.
- Hackman RW, Wagner EL. The Senior Gardening and Nutrition Project (development and transport of a dietary behavior change and health promotion program). J Nutr Educ 1990;22:262-70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3182(12)80104-X.
- Armstrong D. A survey of community gardens in upstate New York: Implications for health promotion and community development. Health Place 2001;6:319-27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1353-8292(00)00013-7.
- Faber M, Phungula MA, Venter SL, Dhansay MA, Benade AJ. Home gardens focusing on the production of yellow and dark-green leafy vegetables increase the serum retinol concentrations of 2–5-y-old children in South Africa. Am J Clin Nutr 2002;76:1048-54.
- Morris JL, Koumjian KL, Briggs M, Zidenberg-Cherr S. Nutrition to grow on: a garden-enhanced nutrition education curriculum for upper-elementary schoolchildren. J Nutr Educ Behav 2002;34:175-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1499-4046(06)60088-2.
- Department for Education . The National Curriculum in England: Framework Document 2013. www.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/210969/NC_framework_document_-_FINAL.pdf (accessed 7 May 2014).
- Bere E, Bjorkelund L. Test-retest reliability of a new self reported comprehensive questionnaire measuring frequencies of different modes of adolescents commuting to school and their parents commuting to work – the ATN questionnaire. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity 2009;6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-6-68.
- Royal Horticultural Society (RHS) . RHS Campaign for School Gardening n.d. http://apps.rhs.org.uk/schoolgardening/default.aspa (accessed 7 May 2014).
- Block K, Gibbs L, Staiger PK, Gold L, Johnson B, Macfarlane S, et al. Growing community: The impact of the Stephanie Alexander Kitchen Garden Program on the social and learning environment in primary schools. Health Educ Behav 2012;39:419-32. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1090198111422937.
- Shaya FT, Flores D, Gbarayor CM, Wang J. School-based obesity interventions: a literature review. J School Health 2008;78:189-96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1746-1561.2008.00285.x.
- Katz D. School-based interventions for health promotion and weight control: not just waiting on the world to change. Annu Rev Public Health 2009;30:253-72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.publhealth.031308.100307.
- Block K, Johnson B, Gibbs L, Staiger P, Townsend M, Macfarlane S, et al. Evaluation of the Stephanie Alexander Kitchen Garden Program. Melbourne: McCaughey Centre; 2009.
- World Health Organization (WHO), Food and Agriculture Organization (FAO) n.d.
- McCormack LA, Laska MN, Larson NI, Story M. Review of the nutritional implications of farmers’ markets and community gardens: A call for evaluation and research efforts. J Am Diet Assoc 2010;110:399-408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2009.11.023.
- Kraak VI, Story M, Swinburn BA. Addressing barriers to improve children’s fruit and vegetable intake. Am J Clin Nutr 2013;97:653-5. http://dx.doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.112.052605.
Appendix 1 Nutrition assessment tools and questionnaires
Child And Diet Evaluation Tool (CADET)
Home food diary
Child knowledge and attitudes questionnaire
School gardening questionnaire
Appendix 2 School recruitment letter for trial 1 schools
Participant information and consent letter to parents for both trials 1 and 2
List of abbreviations
- CADET
- Child And Diet Evaluation Tool
- CI
- confidence interval
- DANTE
- Diet And Nutrition Tool for Evaluation
- EAL
- English as an additional language
- IMDS
- Index of Multiple Deprivation score
- ITT
- intention to treat
- NDNS
- National Diet and Nutrition Survey
- OR
- odds ratio
- RCT
- randomised controlled trial
- RHS
- Royal Horticultural Society
- SCT
- social cognition theory
- SD
- standard deviation
- SE
- standard error
- WHO
- World Health Organization